首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   789篇
  免费   5篇
  国内免费   21篇
教育   405篇
科学研究   82篇
各国文化   3篇
体育   183篇
综合类   74篇
文化理论   1篇
信息传播   67篇
  2023年   4篇
  2022年   11篇
  2021年   14篇
  2020年   13篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   11篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   4篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   28篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   36篇
  2011年   53篇
  2010年   46篇
  2009年   62篇
  2008年   70篇
  2007年   83篇
  2006年   56篇
  2005年   29篇
  2004年   38篇
  2003年   43篇
  2002年   38篇
  2001年   21篇
  2000年   35篇
  1999年   12篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   2篇
  1984年   1篇
  1982年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1957年   1篇
排序方式: 共有815条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
101.
在蒋文野(1985)、周远富(1998)、廖序东(1999)、刘昌海(2001)等学者的研究背景下,在参阅文献的基础上,归纳出《马氏文通》在修辞学领域的研究成果。马氏在微观修辞学领域主要涉及了字的运用、语音节奏的协调、句式的选用;在理论修辞学领域涉及了辞格研究;在应用修辞学领域涉及了语体风格研究。  相似文献   
102.
词族的大量出现,有其独特的语言学价值。它说明了附加构词法表现非常突出,汉语的最佳词音长度受到挑战,某些外来悟睚成为汉语中能产性极强的构词语素。词族现象也为我们提供了透视社会变革,考察人们思想观念变化等方面的认识价值。  相似文献   
103.
《说文》前叙称经说   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
许慎《说文》第十五卷包括许氏自撰的前后两叙和其子许冲的进书表三项内容。其中对前叙的末尾几句,历来学者有不同的见解。许慎所学《孝经》确属古文本。许氏的学术渊源兼有今、古,显示其博通明达,不愧“五经无双”之号。如以古文限之,未免小视许氏了。  相似文献   
104.
分析利用Word 2003文字处理系统自带的“保护文档”和“数字签名”两个功能,实现电子审稿单的真实性保障,并深入探讨其具体操作流程。  相似文献   
105.
基于内嵌Word技术的学报稿件管理系统方案   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
对大学学报稿件管理的业务流程进行了较为全面系统的分析,提出了采用内嵌Word技术解决以往学报稿件管理系统不能很好处理稿件文档的瓶颈,同时基于工作流建立全流程全在线的管理模式,减轻编辑部繁重的工作,并在作者、编辑部、审稿专家之间建立了通畅的管理渠道。  相似文献   
106.
文章主要从"创建文档"、"编辑文档"、"打印文档"等方面叙述了Word在办公应用中的各种技巧使用,对办公应用人员起到了一定的指导作用,有助于提高电脑办公的质量和效率.  相似文献   
107.
毕业论文撰写是每个高校学生的必修环节,本文以电子科技大学学士学位论文撰写要求为例给出了运用Microsoft公司的Word组件完成毕业论文排版的全过程及相关技巧,使得学生能高效、规范地排版毕业论文。  相似文献   
108.
The Simple View of Reading states that reading comprehension is the product of word recognition and listening comprehension. Whereas much research has focused on word recognition accuracy, recent attention has been directed toward word recognition fluency. The current study investigated whether a separate fluency component should be added to the Simple View of Reading. A battery of reading and language measures was administered to 604 children in second, fourth, and eighth grades. Approximately half these children had language and/or nonverbal cognitive impairments in kindergarten, but weighting procedures were used to reduce the potential bias this sampling characteristic may have entailed. Structural equation modeling was used to determine whether fluency accounted for unique variance in reading comprehension after controlling for word recognition accuracy and listening comprehension. Individual profile analyses were conducted to determine the number of individual participants who␣had poor fluency in the spite of good word recognition accuracy and listening comprehension. Results showed that fluency did not account for unique variance in reading␣comprehension and that few individuals had problems in fluency separate from word recognition accuracy or listening comprehension. Thus, it does not appear that a separate fluency component should be added to the Simple View of Reading.  相似文献   
109.
The role of spelling recognition was examined in word reading skills and reading comprehension for dyslexic and nondyslexic children. Dyslexic and nondyslexic children were matched on their raw word reading proficiency. Relationships between spelling recognition and the following were examined for both groups of children: verbal ability, working memory, phonological measures, rapid naming, word reading, and reading comprehension. Children’s performance in spelling recognition was significantly associated with their skills in word reading and reading comprehension regardless of their reading disability status. Furthermore, spelling recognition contributed significant variance to reading comprehension for both dyslexic and nondyslexic children after the effects of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and word reading proficiency had been accounted for. The results support the role of spelling recognition in reading development for both groups of children and they are discussed using a componential reading fluency framework.  相似文献   
110.
The purpose of the experiments was to determine the automatic use of large or small word reading units in young readers in the absence of word decoding strategies. Picture-word Stroop interference was examined from four types of conflicting labels: (a) words containing both highly predictable grapheme–phoneme correspondence (GPC) units and highly consistent rime units (henceforth, Hi-GPC + Hi-Rime); (b) words with highly predictable GPC units and less consistent rime units (Hi-GPC + Lo-Rime); (c) words with low predictability GPC units and highly consistent rime units (Low GPC + High Rime); (d) nonwords that contained both highly predictable GPC and highly consistent rime units. Naming time for pictures containing these labels was compared against that for pictures with random letter strings or no labels. In Experiment 1, Stroop interference was examined in first, second, and third grade children to determine whether there was developmental change in the presence of rime or GPC interference. In Experiment 2, Stroop interference was examined as a function of relative reading skill in first grade children. In Experiment 3, Stroop interference in adults was compared to the use of rime or GPC pronunciation strategies for nonword reading. In all experiments, Stroop interference in picture naming was longer for pictures with highly predictable GPC unit labels than less predictable GPC unit labels. However, in Experiment 3, even though adults showed interference from predictable GPC units in the Stroop task, they always preferred rime pronunciation for ambiguous nonwords in the nonword reading task. It is argued that the current experiments provide evidence for a flexible units model. The results of this study were presented at the Cognitive Development Society meeting, November 2001, Virginia Beach, VA, and the American Educational Research Association meeting, April 2004, San Diego, California.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号