共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
In a Pavlovian conditioning situation, an initially neutral stimulus may be made excitatory by nonreinforced presentations in compound with an established conditioned excitor [i.e., second-order conditioning (SOC)]. The established excitor may be either a punctate cue or the training context. In four conditioned suppression experiments using rats, we investigated whether SOC phenomena parallel other cue interaction effects. In Experiment 1, we found that the response potential of a target stimulus was directly related to the intertrial interval when SOC was mediated by a punctate cue, and inversely related to the intertrial interval when SOC was mediated by the training context. Experiment 2 demonstrated that punctate- and context-mediated SOC are oppositely affected by posttraining context extinction, and Experiments 3 and 4 demonstrated that context- and punctate-mediated SOC are differentially affected by conditioned stimulus (Experiment 3) and unconditioned stimulus (Experiment 4) preexposure treatments. These findings parallel phenomena in conditioned inhibition and cue competition situations. 相似文献
2.
Lick suppression experiments with rats revealed that the magnitude of both second-order conditioning (Experiment 1) and sensory preconditioning (Experiment 2) was superior when that conditioning was based on backward (US→CS) relative to forward (CS→US) first-order pairings of a CS and US. The superiority of backward relative to forward first-order conditioning on suppression to the higher order cues can be understood by assuming that the magnitude of higher order conditioning was determined by a memory representation of the higher order cues that provided information about the expected temporal location of the US. The results suggest that temporal information such as order between paired CSs and USs was encoded, preserved, and integrated with memory for the higher order stimuli. The relevance of these findings to memory integration in Pavlovian learning, the temporal coding hypothesis (Barnet, Arnold, & Miller, 1991; Matzel, Held, & Miller, 1988), backward excitatory conditioning, and the associative structure that underlies second-order Pavlovian fear conditioning are discussed. 相似文献
3.
Two experiments were conducted to determine if contextual stimuli used as S2 in a higher-order differential conditioning procedure would control the performance of rats. Discrete stimuli were first paired with footshock in a separate training context. During second-order training, a shock-associated discrete stimulus was presented in one of two discriminable observation chambers. Over 4 days of training, subjects engaged in more freezing in the context associated with an excitatory discrete S1, relative to a context in which no discrete stimulus, or a stimulus that had been explicitly unpaired with shock delivery, was presented. After acquisition of the second-order discrimination, animals were returned to the original training context where they received a “signaled inflation” treatment designed to change the current value of S1, and the US. This postconditioning manipulation did not selectively affect performance of defensive freezing or conditional analgesia in S2. 相似文献
4.
According to the temporal-coding hypothesis (TCH; Savastano & Miller, Behavioural Processes 44:147–162, 1998), acquired associations include temporal information concerning the interval between the associated elements. Moreover, the TCH posits that subjects can integrate two independently acquired associations that share a common element (e.g., S2–S1 and S1–US), which results in the creation of a third association with its own temporal relationship (S2–US). Some evidence has suggested that such temporal integration occurs at the time of testing (Molet, Miguez, Cham, & Miller, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 38:369–380, 2012). Here we report two fear-conditioning experiments with rats conducted to identify the associative structure of the integrated temporal relationship. The goal was to distinguish between two possible associative structures that could exist following an initial test on which temporal integration occurs: (1) Conditioned responding to S2 on subsequent tests could be the result of recurring successive activation of two independently learned temporal maps that remain independently stored in memory (i.e., S2–S1 plus S1–US). (2) Temporal integration at the moment of initial testing could result in the formation of a direct S2–US (or S2–response) temporal map. Integration was found to occur at test and to produce a new association that was independent of associations with the common element (S1). However, the associative status of S1 appeared to modulate whether or not the new association with S2 was US-specific (S2–US) or directly activated a fear response (S2–response). 相似文献
5.
6.
Second-order conditioning was examined using the rabbit eyeblink paradigm and the gerbil CER paradigm. Pavlov’s hypothesis that stimulus overlap on second-order trials produces conditioned inhibition and that nonoverlap leads to second-order conditioning was not confirmed. Our results also revealed that the manner in which first-order and second-order trials are intermixed has an important influence on the properties of the second-order CS. A within-session mixture of first- and second-order trials tended to produce second-order conditioning, and a between-session mixture tended to produce conditioned inhibition. Second-order conditioning was more prominent with the gerbil fear response than with the rabbit eyelid response. 相似文献
7.
The signaling function of the second-order CS (S2) was manipulated in second-order autoshaping by arranging a partial reinforcement schedule. S2 was paired with a well-conditioned first-order CS (SI) on a continuous reinforcement or a 25% reinforcement schedule in different groups. Schedule of reinforcement did not influence the number of S2-S1 pairings required to establish keypecking to S2. However, in the postacquisition sessions, responding to S2 was initially weaker but persisted for many more sessions on the 25% schedule than on the 100% schedule. The data indicate that S2-S1 pairings are responsible both for the acquisition of second-order keypecking to S2 and for the subsequent conversion of S2 into an inhibitory stimulus. 相似文献
8.
Two conditioned lick suppression experiments with rats used feature-negative training (A-footshock trials intermixed with
nonreinforced XA presentations) to analyze the role of the number of XA compound presentations and the temporal relationship
of the elements within the compound (simultaneous or serial) as determinants of the resulting behavioral control. Second-order
conditioning (i.e., excitatory behavioral control by X) was observed to decline as the number of XA compound trials was increased.
This decline was more rapid if X and A were presented simultaneously, as opposed to serially (i.e., X before A; Experiment
1). Conditioned inhibition to X, as assessed by a summation test (Experiment 1) and a retardation test (Experiment 2), increased
with the number of XA trials and did so more quickly for simultaneous than for serial pairings of X and A. The results help
to clarify previously discrepant findings regarding factors that promote excitation versus inhibition with this protocol. 相似文献
9.
Steinmetz AB Skosnik PD Edwards CR Bolbecker AR Steinmetz JE Hetrick WP 《Learning & behavior》2011,39(4):358-370
Delay eye-blink conditioning is an associative learning task that can be utilized to probe the functional integrity of the cerebellum and related neural circuits. Typically, a single interstimulus interval (ISI) is utilized, and the amplitude of the conditioned response (CR) is the primary dependent variable. To study the timing of the CR, an ISI shift can be introduced (e.g., shifting the ISI from 350 to 850 ms). In each phase, a conditioned stimulus (e.g., a 400- or 900-ms tone) coterminates with a 50-ms corneal air puff unconditioned stimulus. The ability of a subject to adjust the CR to the changing ISI constitutes a critical timing shift. The feasibility of this procedure was examined in healthy human participants (N = 58) using a bidirectional ISI shift procedure while cortical event-related brain potentials were measured. CR acquisition was faster and the responses better timed when a short ISI was used. After the ISI shift, additional training was necessary to allow asymptotic responding at the new ISI. Interestingly, auditory event-related potentials to the CR were not associated with conditioning measures at either ISI. 相似文献
10.
教师在词汇教学中运用联想归纳的方法,不仅有助于学生快捷地掌握大量的词汇,而且能激发学生的学习兴趣。本作介绍了在教学中经常用到的几种方法,供大家参考。 相似文献
11.
12.
Are humans unique in their ability to interpret exogenous events as causes? We addressed this question by observing the behavior of rats for indications of causal learning. Within an operant motor–sensory preconditioning paradigm, associative surgical techniques revealed that rats attempted to control an outcome (i.e., a potential effect) by manipulating a potential exogenous cause (i.e., an intervention). Rats were able to generate an innocuous auditory stimulus. This stimulus was then paired with an aversive stimulus. The animals subsequently avoided potential generation of the predictive cue, but not if the aversive stimulus was subsequently devalued or the predictive cue was extinguished (Exp. 1). In Experiment 2, we demonstrated that the aversive stimulus we used was in fact aversive, that it was subject to devaluation, that the cue–aversive stimulus pairings did make the cue a conditioned stimulus, and that the cue was subject to extinction. In Experiments 3 and 4, we established that the decrease in leverpressing observed in Experiment 1 was goal-directed instrumental behavior rather than purely a product of Pavlovian conditioning. To the extent that interventions suggest causal reasoning, it appears that causal reasoning can be based on associations between contiguous exogenous events. Thus, contiguity appears capable of establishing causal relationships between exogenous events. Our results challenge the widely held view that causal learning is uniquely human, and suggest that causal learning is explicable in an associative framework. 相似文献
13.
14.
词语语义可分为概念意义和联想意义,联想意义与社会化密切相关。同一指称概念的词语会产生附加在概念意义之上的不同的联想意义。从社会化视角研究英汉词语联想意义的差异有助于理解联想意义的化制约性和交际价值性。提高跨化交际的能力, 相似文献
15.
林振环 《张家口职业技术学院学报》2002,15(4):36-38
重视体育教学活动交往性的特点,有利于促进体育教学育人功能的正常发挥,有利于体育教学中打破知识授受的单一模式。有意识地加强师生间、学生间的交往,可以促进学生的良好发展。 相似文献
16.
本文引进BCI-代数的结合理想,讨论了它的性质,得到理想成为结合理想的充要条件。借此给出了结合BCI-代数的理想刻画,BCI-代数结合的充要条件是它的每个理想是结合的。 相似文献
17.
李金龙 《陕西理工学院学报(社会科学版)》2001,(3)
给出了两种求广义结合BCI_代数商代数的十分方便的方法 ,即 :设H为广义结合BCI_代数X的子代数 ,(1 ) x∈X ,令xH ={x h|h∈H} ,X/H ={xH|x∈X} ,定义xH yH =(x y)H ,则 (X/H ; ,0H)是广义结合BCI_代数 ;(2 ) x∈X ,令Hx={ (x h) h|h∈H} ,X/H2 ={Hx|x∈X} ,定义Hx Hy=Hx y,则 (X/H2 ; ,H0 )是广义结合BCI_代数 . 相似文献
18.
19.
任艳丽 《鞍山师范学院学报》1999,(1)
进一步讨论拟零化根 NQ ,证明了 NQ 是 Amitsur- Kurosh根 ,得到了 NQ -半单环的结构定理 ,给出了 NQ -根的模刻划 . 相似文献