共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
In two experiments, rats were given intermixed or blocked preexposure to two similar compound stimuli, AX and BX. Following preexposure, conditioning trials took place in which AX (Experiment 1) or a novel compound stimulus NX (Experiment 2) was paired with a food-unconditioned stimulus in an appetitive Pavlovian preparation. Animals that were given alternated preexposure showed lower generalization from AX to BX (Experiment 1) and from NX to a new compound, ZX (Experiment 2), than animals that were given blocked preexposure, a perceptual learning and a perceptual learning transfer effect, respectively. 相似文献
2.
In three experiments that used appetitive preparations with rats, we examined the effects of reinforcing a compound consisting of two previously reinforced stimuli on subsequent responding to those stimuli. Experiment 1 showed that a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus given this treatment evoked fewer magazine entries when presented alone than did a reinforced stimulus that did not receive the compound treatment. Experiment 2 examined inhibition of delay and generalization decrement accounts for the results of Experiment 1. Experiment 3 extended this finding to an instrumental learning paradigm. 相似文献
3.
Edward Zamble 《Learning & behavior》1974,2(2):101-105
A CS was paired with rewarding intracranial stimulation while Ss were immobilized by curare. Tests of the activity response to the CS were carried out before and after conditioning. With either simple or discriminative conditioning, activity to the rewarded CS increased after training. These results are comparable to those in studies using food reward and support the hypothesis that a central motivational state is conditioned to stimuli preceding reward. 相似文献
4.
We used an appetitive sensory preconditioning procedure to investigate temporal integration in rats in two experiments. In
Phase 1, rats were presented with simultaneous compound trials on which a 10-sec conditioned stimulus (CS) X was embedded
within a 60-sec CS A. In Group Early, CS X occurred during the early portion of CS A, whereas in Group Late, CS X occurred
during the latter portion of CS A. In Phase 2, CS X was paired simultaneously with sucrose. On a subsequent test with CS A,
the rate of magazine entries peaked during the early portions of the stimulus in Group Early and during the latter portions
of the stimulus in Group Late (Experiments 1 and 2). Similar response peaks were not observed on tests with a control stimulus
that had been presented in compound with a stimulus that did not signal reward (Experiment 2). 相似文献
5.
In three experiments, rats received appetitive conditioning with two unconditioned stimuli (US+ and US*) that supported a similar conditioned response. Stimulus A was first paired with US+, and then, for a second stage of training, the compound AB was paired with US*. Subsequent test trials with A revealed an abnormally strong conditioned response. The findings from additional groups indicated that such enhanced conditioning with A depended on the presence of B in the second stage and on the use of US+ and US* for the two training stages. A modification to a configural theory of conditioning is proposed in order to explain the results. 相似文献
6.
In three experiments, rats received presentations of a diffuse 30-sec stimulus (a light) and of food, and their tendency to enter the food tray was monitored. Experiment 1 showed that when the light was made to signal the delivery of food, the response of entering the food tray increased in frequency during the stimulus. The acquisition of this conditioned response to the light was retarded in subjects that had received preexposure to the stimulus. In Experiments 2 and 3, subjects received preexposure to the stimulus, some in the same context as that subsequently used for stimulus-food pairings and some in a different context. Those experiencing the change of context acquired the response more readily. It is argued that these results demonstrate a latent inhibition effect that is attenuated by contextual change. 相似文献
7.
Conditioning trials that are massed in time produce less conditioning than those that are spaced in time. Four experiments
with rat subjects examined whether a recent conditioning trial interferes with conditioning on the next trial by temporarily
“priming” information in short-term memory (e.g., Wagner, 1978, 1981). We used appetitive conditioning procedures in which
priming trials preceded target trials by 60 sec. When the priming trials were nonreinforced presentations of a conditioned
stimulus (CS), the CS had to be the same CS as the one on the target trial to interfere with conditioning. When priming trials
were actual CS-unconditioned stimulus (US) pairings, the CS identity did not matter; the US was the event that interfered
with conditioning on the next trial. Reinforced trials reduced performance in a way that did not depend on context blocking.
The results suggest that CS and US priming effects do contribute to conditioning deficits observed with massed trial procedures.
The results are consistent with Wagner’s (1981) “sometimes opponent process,” or SOP, model, although a result that is paradoxical
for the model suggests that recent USs may have motivational as well as memory effects. 相似文献
8.
Two experiments examined the summation and transfer of negative occasion setting in serial feature negative (X→A?/A+) and negative patterning discriminations (X→A?/X+/A+) in a discrete-trial operant task with rats. With both discrimination procedures, negative occasion setting transferred readily (but not perfectly) to cues trained as targets in another similar discrimination procedure, but not to cues that were separately trained. Furthermore, with both discrimination procedures, the negative occasion-setting powers of features from two individual discriminations summed, both when those features were trained with the same targets and when they were trained with different targets. After negative patterning discriminations, in which the feature (X) cues were separately reinforced, this summation of negative occasion setting occurred despite the concomitant summation of the features’ excitatory control in the absence of an explicit target cue. These data, which replicated and extended previously reported data from Pavlovian feature negative procedures, are discussed in the context of hierarchical, generalization, and network models of discrimination learning. 相似文献
9.
Andrew R. Delamater 《Learning & behavior》1995,23(1):31-39
The outcome-selective effects of presenting intertrial unconditioned stimuli (USs) in a rat appetitive conditioning paradigm were examined in two experiments. In both experiments, two stimuli were paired with different outcomes, while one of these outcomes was also presented in the intertrial interval (A+, B*, +). Two measures of learning, stimulus-elicited magazine approach and Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer, were used to examine these effects. The presence of freely occurring outcomes in the intertrial interval (ITI) was observed to interfere more with the learning of a new association (Experiment 1) and to degrade more an already established association (Experiment 2) when the conditioned stimulus had been paired with the same outcome as that occurring in the ITI. An outcome-selective effect of ITI USs distinguishes among accounts of contingency based on general performance, attentional, and motivational mechanisms from those based on more specific associative mechanisms. Overall, the data highlight the importance of specific encoding processes in the analysis of associative learning. 相似文献
10.
Peter C. Holland 《Learning & behavior》1986,14(2):111-120
Three experiments examined the acquisition and transfer of Pavlovian feature-positive discriminations (XA+, A?) in rat subjects. To identify the nature of the associations formed in those discriminations, the form of the conditioned responses (CRs) was examined. If the feature,X, and common element, A, cues started and ended together onXA compoud trials, associations betweenX and the food unconditioned stimulus (US) were acquired. If the onsets and/or terminations ofX preceded those of A,X acquired the ability to set the occasion for responding to A, that is, A evoked CRs only onXA compound trials. The acquisition of occasion setting was favored when (1) the onset of X preceded that of A, (2) the interval betweenX and A and/or the US was relatively long, and (3) the termination ofX occurred prior to the onset of A. The occasion-setting power ofX was fairly specific to A:X did not modulate responding evoked by another cue that had been first trained and then extinguished or by a cue that had been paired with the US only a few times. However,X did enhance responding to a cue that had been a common element in another, identical feature-positive discrimination. That transfer was somewhat greater if theX and A terminated together than ifX terminated prior to the onset of A. Implications for theories of stimulus control in Pavlovian conditioning are discussed. 相似文献
11.
Peter C. Holland 《Learning & behavior》1979,7(4):424-432
The form of rats’ Pavlovian conditioned responses to visual and auditory conditioned stimuli (CSs) paired with a variety of unconditioned stimuli (USs) was examined in three experiments using direct behavioral observation techniques. In Experiment 1, the form of conditioned behavior occurring most frequently during later portions of the CS-US interval depended only on which of several appetitive USs was used, but the form of behavior occurring most frequently during early portions of the CS-US interval depended only on the nature of the CS. US-dependent behaviors resembled the response to the US, and CS-dependent behaviors resembled the original orienting response (OR) to the CS. In Experiment 2, the use of larger magnitude appetitive USs resulted in higher frequencies of US-dependent behaviors, but lower frequencies of CS-dependent behaviors in the presence of auditory and visual CSs. In Experiment 3, US-dependent conditioned behavior to auditory and visual CSs paired with shock was more frequent when high-intensity shocks were used, but CS-dependent behavior was more frequent when low-intensity shocks were used. These results suggested that Pavlovian conditioned responding may involve two independent types of behavior—one appropriate to the US and another based on the original OR to the CS. 相似文献
12.
Raymond E. Webster 《Psychology in the schools》1976,13(1):72-76
This study uses time-out as the primary therapeutic intervention with a 13-year-old highly aggressive male in a public school setting. Specific behaviors were listed prior to beginning TO and were explained to the child and teachers. TO was used over a 10-week period with extinction of the specified behaviors occurring after the seventh week. During an 8-week follow-up only three instances of the aggressive behaviors were observed. During the third and fourth weeks of TO the child began to show spontaneous interest in school work. Statistical analysis by an interrupted time-series program revealed a t (102) = - 8.290 (p <.0005) for change in level between baseline observations with intervention and follow-up data. It is concluded that TO is a sufficiently intense aversive event of itself to alter some types of deviant behavior without direct application of positive reinforcers. It is most propitiously used when alternative response modes are made available for the child. 相似文献
13.
R. F. S. Job 《Learning & behavior》1987,15(3):342-346
Four groups of rats were exposed to response-contingent, yoked noncontingent, or en masse food deliveries in a Skinner box or to no experimental pretreatment. All groups were subsequently tested for transfer of the learned-helplessness effect to an appetitive discrete-trial T-maze discrimination employing experimenter-blind procedures. The yoked group showed retarded discrimination learning in comparison with the response-contingent and naive control groups but not in comparison with the en masse control group. This nonsignificant difference between the yoked group and the en masse group may reflect the effect of limited exposure to uncontrollability in the en masse group. The groups did not differ in terms of the speed of maze traversal. The latter result suggests that the learned-helplessness effect observed in discrimination was not due to a competing response. 相似文献
14.
Conditioned responding to a well-trained conditioned stimulus (CS) was assessed in an eyelid conditioning situation under three conditions of prior stimulation: when preceded by recent presentation of the same CS, when preceded by recent presentation of a different CS, and when preceded by no recent stimulation. As compared to the latter condition, prior presentation of the same CS depressed responding, while prior presentation of a different CS augmented responding. Both effects diminished as the interval separating the test stimulus from the preceding CS was lengthened over the range of 2.5 to 10 sec. The results were discussed in terms of the interaction of short-term memory representations with subsequent stimulation, and a specific theory concerning the effects of “priming.” 相似文献
15.
Three experiments with rat subjects examined the effects of contextual stimuli on performance in appetitive conditioning. A 10-sec tone conditioned stimulus (CS) was paired with a food-pellet unconditioned stimulus (US); conditioning was indexed by the observation of headjerking, a response of the rat to auditory stimuli associated with food. In Experiment 1, a context switch following initial conditioning did not affect conditioned responding to the tone; however, when the response was extinguished in the different context, a return to the original conditioning context “renewed” extinguished responding. These results were replicated in Experiments 2 and 3 after equating exposure to the two contexts (Experiment 2) and massing the conditioning and extinction trials (Experiment 3). The results of Experiment 1 also demonstrated that separate exposure to the US following extinction reinstates extinguished responding to the tone; this effect was further shown to depend at least partly on presenting the US in the context in which testing is to occur (Experiments 2 and 3). Overall, the results are consistent with previous data from aversive conditioning procedures. In either appetitive or aversive conditioning, the context may be especially important in affecting performance after extinction. 相似文献
16.
17.
The present study employed a Pavlovian-instrumental-transfer paradigm to investigate the role of conditioned fear in appetitive discrimination learning. Each of three Pavlovian training procedures was used to establish a conditioned fear excitor (CS+), a “neutral” CS (CSo), and a conditioned fear inhibitor (CS?). Then, the CSs were administered to rats in the three groups contingent upon the rewarded response in a difficult visual discrimination. In addition, half of each group received shock punishment for each incorrect response. Relative to CSo, CS+ facilitated performance in contrast to the usual interfering effect of conditioned suppressors; conversely, CS? retarded performance even when its reinforcing action (fear inhibition) was potentiated by punishment for the incorrect response. These results, together with other findings showing a reversed outcome when the CSs are administered for the incorrect response, indicate that Pavlovian conditioning comprises both general signaling and affective functions, the former reflecting a basic “expectancy” or nominal type of cognitive processing in the rat. 相似文献
18.
This study investigates the effects of exposure to inescapable shock on the acquisition of a low-activity appetitive response using a trial procedure. Inescapable shock was found to interfere with the acquisition of a nose-poke response to obtain food as compared with animals exposed to either escapable shock or no shock. In addition, general activity levels were measured separately during the trial and the intertrial interval during the appetitive test. Inescapably shocked animals were less active during the trial component than were either the escapably shocked or the nonshocked animals. However, no differential levels of activity were observed during the intertriai interval component of the appetitive test. The relevance of these findings for both the learned helplessness and the learned inactivity hypotheses is discussed. 相似文献
19.
Peter C. Holland 《Learning & behavior》2000,28(2):121-135
The effects of trial (T) and intertrial (I) durations were examined in two Pavlovian conditioning experiments with rats, in which a noise conditioned stimulus (CS)
was paired with food delivery. In Experiment 1,T was either 10 or 20 sec, andI ranged from 15 to 960 sec, in separate groups of rats. The acquisition rate and final level of conditioned responding showed
ratio invariance: They were better predicted by theI/T ratio than byI orT alone. In Experiment 2, theI/T ratio was 6.0 in all the groups, andT was 20, 40, 80, or 160 sec. Ratio invariance was not observed: Despite the commonI/T ratio, the rate of acquisition, final level of conditioned responding, and the ability of the CS to block conditioning of
another stimulus differed among the groups. At the same time, the temporal distribution of conditioned responding withinT was similar in all the groups throughout conditioning and extinction and showed superpositioning when normalized acrossT. Many but not all aspects of the data were consistent with scalar timing theory. 相似文献