首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   23994篇
  免费   288篇
  国内免费   16篇
教育   17046篇
科学研究   1949篇
各国文化   260篇
体育   2355篇
综合类   8篇
文化理论   219篇
信息传播   2461篇
  2021年   240篇
  2020年   367篇
  2019年   565篇
  2018年   757篇
  2017年   686篇
  2016年   681篇
  2015年   417篇
  2014年   552篇
  2013年   4329篇
  2012年   506篇
  2011年   563篇
  2010年   421篇
  2009年   447篇
  2008年   493篇
  2007年   459篇
  2006年   425篇
  2005年   377篇
  2004年   394篇
  2003年   312篇
  2002年   330篇
  2001年   465篇
  2000年   512篇
  1999年   440篇
  1998年   259篇
  1997年   251篇
  1996年   307篇
  1995年   245篇
  1994年   260篇
  1993年   224篇
  1992年   351篇
  1991年   372篇
  1990年   344篇
  1989年   366篇
  1988年   340篇
  1987年   328篇
  1986年   328篇
  1985年   373篇
  1984年   294篇
  1983年   304篇
  1982年   240篇
  1981年   247篇
  1980年   260篇
  1979年   350篇
  1978年   259篇
  1977年   240篇
  1976年   201篇
  1975年   181篇
  1974年   183篇
  1973年   176篇
  1971年   174篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
171.
172.
Two experiments are reported comparing performance in dominant and submissive rats as determined by a food-competition procedure. Ss in Experiment I were either trained to criterion or overtrained before being reversed on a visual discrimination task. Although the two groups did not differ significantly in learning the initial task, the dominant Ss were significantly slower on reversal than the submissives. Experiment II, which utilized a tandem runway, showed that when the reinforcer which maintained the running response was not available in the first goalbox, submissive rats responded with a greater increase in running speed in the second runway than dominant rats. These findings were discussed as reflecting motivational differences between dominant and submissive rats.  相似文献   
173.
To study sensitivity to radial location of an odor source, 20 human newborns, ranging from 16 to 130 hours of age, were presented with a small amount of ammonium hydroxide. The odor source was placed near the nose slightly to the left or right of midline, with its position randomized over repeated trails. Direction of headturn with respect to the odor location and diffuse motor activity were scored from the videotape recordings of the newborns' behavior. It was found that as a group, the newborns turned away from the odor source more frequently than they turned toward it. The tendency to turn away from the odor was stronger in infants who displayed less motor activity after the response. Newborns also exhibited a right bias in the direction of the head movements. It is concluded that a spatially appropriate avoidance response is present in the neonate and that the newborn is innately sensitive to the radial location of an odor.  相似文献   
174.
175.
Interaction and learning: Theorizing on the art of teaching   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
176.
177.
178.
179.
Do infants see emotional expressions in static faces?   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
To determine whether young infants discriminate photographs of different emotions on an affect-relevant basis or on the basis of isolated features unrelated to emotion, groups of 17-, 23-, and 29-week-olds were habituated to slides of 8 women posing either Toothy Angry, Nontoothy Angry, or Nontoothy Smiling facial expressions and were then shown 2 new women in the familiarized expression and in a novel Toothy Smiling expression. At all 3 ages, recovery to the novel Toothy Smiling faces occurred only after habituation to Nontoothy faces (whether smiling or angry), not after habituation to Toothy Angry faces, indicating that infants had been responsive to nonspecific features of the photographs (presence or absence of bared teeth) rather than to affectively relevant configurations of features. In a second experiment, 2 older age groups (35 and 41 weeks) also proved to be insensitive to affect-related aspects of still faces, though more so for angry than for happy expressions. It is suggested that the young infant's difficulty in extracting emotional information from static stimuli may be attributable to the absence of the critical invariants (dynamic, multimodally specified) that characterize naturalistic expressions of emotion.  相似文献   
180.
The purpose of this study was to quantify social interaction rhythms in 3- and 5-month-old term and preterm infants and their mothers. Infant-mother dyads were videotaped in a 3-min face-to-face paradigm. For each second of the interaction, separate scores were assigned to infant and mother indicating levels of affective involvement, creating 2 180-sec time series. Spectral and cross-spectral techniques were used to quantify periodicities in each member of the dyad and to estimate the synchrony or coherence of interactional rhythms between each infant and mother. Results showed the existence of periodicities in the behavior of each infant and mother at 3 and 5 months, with most subjects showing spectral peaks between .022 and .10 Hz. Increases from 3 to 5 months in behavioral periodicities were found for infants and mothers as well as for the coherence between infant-mother dyads. Term dyads showed higher coherence than preterm dyads at both 3 and 5 months. Term infants more often led the interaction at both ages. These results were taken as evidence that behavioral periodicities, which may be biologically based, underlie early mother-infant interaction and provide a temporal structure for the organization of cognitive and affective experience. Differences in synchrony between term and preterm infants may explain later reported differences in language between these groups.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号