首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4939篇
  免费   65篇
  国内免费   9篇
教育   3633篇
科学研究   385篇
各国文化   60篇
体育   470篇
综合类   19篇
文化理论   104篇
信息传播   342篇
  2023年   28篇
  2022年   85篇
  2021年   135篇
  2020年   177篇
  2019年   263篇
  2018年   335篇
  2017年   329篇
  2016年   272篇
  2015年   193篇
  2014年   225篇
  2013年   916篇
  2012年   243篇
  2011年   171篇
  2010年   147篇
  2009年   120篇
  2008年   124篇
  2007年   105篇
  2006年   90篇
  2005年   112篇
  2004年   112篇
  2003年   126篇
  2002年   118篇
  2001年   83篇
  2000年   56篇
  1999年   35篇
  1998年   24篇
  1997年   14篇
  1996年   27篇
  1995年   28篇
  1994年   22篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   18篇
  1991年   20篇
  1990年   19篇
  1989年   20篇
  1988年   10篇
  1985年   9篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   12篇
  1980年   15篇
  1979年   8篇
  1978年   9篇
  1977年   10篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   8篇
  1973年   7篇
  1966年   7篇
  1963年   8篇
排序方式: 共有5013条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
This study examined the effect of (a) high- and low-level questions and (b) reading the text before the questions asked on performance, delayed text recall, and deep text comprehension, as well as on specific text-inspection patterns. Participants were 37 undergraduate students who answered either high- or low-level questions using the software Read&Answer to read and answer questions on the computer screen. Additionally, half of the sample read first a text and then answered the questions (reading-first condition), whereas the other half answered the questions without having read the text in advance (no-reading-first condition). All participants had the text available to search for the answer. Results indicated that high-level questions facilitated deep comprehension but not immediate performance or delayed recall of text, independently of the reading condition, and that high- and low-level questions differentially affected text-inspection patterns.  相似文献   
122.
The main purposes of this research were twofold. We examined the samenesses about learning disabilities (LD) in Guatemala and Spain, two countries with the same language but cultural, political, and educational differences, first analyzing data about the prevalence of reading and spelling disabilities in Guatemala City and the Spanish region of the Canary Islands. The focus of the second study was to determine whether there are cross‐national patterns of significant differences in cognitive processes associated with reading and spelling disabilities from a developmental approach in these two cultural contexts. We found some differences in the prevalence of specific LD in reading between both countries but we did not find significant differences between Guatemalan and Spanish reading‐disabled children in cognitive processes that are involved in reading and spelling acquisition in spite of the cultural and educational differences between the two countries.  相似文献   
123.
124.
125.
126.
127.
Theory-of-mind (ToM) abilities were studied in 176 deaf children aged 3 years 11 months to 8 years 3 months who use either American Sign Language (ASL) or oral English, with hearing parents or deaf parents. A battery of tasks tapping understanding of false belief and knowledge state and language skills, ASL or English, was given to each child. There was a significant delay on ToM tasks in deaf children of hearing parents, who typically demonstrate language delays, regardless of whether they used spoken English or ASL. In contrast, deaf children from deaf families performed identically to same-aged hearing controls (N=42). Both vocabulary and understanding syntactic complements were significant independent predictors of success on verbal and low-verbal ToM tasks.  相似文献   
128.
129.
Average-ability students often do not take full advantage of learning in mixed-ability groups because they hardly engage in the group interaction. This study examined whether structuring collaboration by group roles and ground rules for helping behavior might help overcome this participatory inequality. In a plant biology course, heterogeneously grouped fourth-grade boys (n=164) were randomly assigned to a structured collaboration condition or an unconstrained comparison condition. Results indicated positive effects of structured collaboration on average-ability students’ achievement, motivation, and contribution to the group interaction. Another positive result was that structuring collaboration did not lower the scores of the high and low-ability students on these measures.  相似文献   
130.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号