首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   15668篇
  免费   229篇
  国内免费   119篇
教育   10775篇
科学研究   2048篇
各国文化   112篇
体育   1370篇
综合类   11篇
文化理论   120篇
信息传播   1580篇
  2021年   115篇
  2020年   231篇
  2019年   329篇
  2018年   453篇
  2017年   409篇
  2016年   405篇
  2015年   259篇
  2014年   326篇
  2013年   2666篇
  2012年   302篇
  2011年   338篇
  2010年   246篇
  2009年   246篇
  2008年   257篇
  2007年   289篇
  2006年   276篇
  2005年   274篇
  2004年   254篇
  2003年   250篇
  2002年   239篇
  2001年   255篇
  2000年   272篇
  1999年   274篇
  1998年   142篇
  1997年   149篇
  1996年   183篇
  1995年   169篇
  1994年   132篇
  1993年   142篇
  1992年   216篇
  1991年   223篇
  1990年   230篇
  1989年   258篇
  1988年   210篇
  1987年   225篇
  1986年   220篇
  1985年   250篇
  1984年   222篇
  1983年   212篇
  1982年   176篇
  1981年   168篇
  1980年   158篇
  1979年   245篇
  1978年   191篇
  1977年   180篇
  1976年   148篇
  1975年   132篇
  1974年   151篇
  1973年   132篇
  1971年   120篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
972.
Converging evidence from a number of neuroimaging studies, including our own, suggest that fluent word identification in reading is related to the functional integrity of two left hemisphere posterior systems: a temporo-parietal system and a ventral occipito-temporal system. These posterior systems are functionally disrupted in developmental dyslexia. Reading disabled, relative to nonimpaired, readers demonstrate heightened reliance on both inferior frontal and right hemisphere posterior regions, presumably in compensation for the LH posterior difficulties. We propose a neurobiological account suggesting that for normally developing readers the temporo-parietal system predominates at first, and is associated with aspects of processing critical in learning to integrate orthography with phonological and lexical-semantic features of printed words. The occipito-temporal system, by contrast, constitutes a fast, late-developing, word-identification system that underlies fluent word recognition in skilled readers.  相似文献   
973.
974.
975.
976.
977.
978.
1 Introduction Sincethediscoveryofgiantmagnetoresistance(GMR),magneticmaterialswithrandommixtureofferromagneticandantiferromagneticpropertieshaverecentlyrecievedalotofattention[1].ThemagneticpropertiesofFeandCrarequitedifferentfromeachother.Feisferromagneticbelow1043K,whileCrisantiferromagneticatroomtemperature.GMRexistesnotonlyinsandwichandmultilayerfilms,butalsoinothernanostructuralmaterials,suchasgranularfilmsandnanostructurebulkmaterialspreparedbymechanicalalloying(MA).MAhasbeendev…  相似文献   
979.
Two groups of 10 novice subjects each were trained to perform attacking forehand drives in table tennis and land the balls as fast and as accurately as possible onto a target on the opposite side of the net under two different training conditions. Under the static training condition, the balls were to be struck from a constant position, and under the dynamic training condition, balls approached the subjects in a normal way. Both groups were tested under dynamic conditions prior to and after four days of training, during which they received 1,600 practice trials. Both groups of subjects were shown to increase the number of balls that landed on the target, and learning was also evident from an increased consistency of the direction of travel of the bat at the moment of ball/bat contact. However, no increase in consistency was found for the location of the bat at the moment of ball/bat contact and for the movement times. Thus, learning can occur in the absence of externally generated time-to-contact information, but this is not due to the establishment of a consistent movement form. Learning appears to progress from control at the moment of ball/bat contact backward, toward the moment of initiation.  相似文献   
980.
The research literature on educational constructivism is voluminous (see the Carmichael (1990) Pfundt & Duit (1994) and Driver et al. (1994b) bibliographies cited below). The research - in both the Piagetian and Alternative Conception traditions - covers children's learning, cognitive development, curriculum development, classroom practices, teacher education, and much else. There is a further enormous literature on constructivism in philosophy of science (see Leplin (1984) and Churchland & Hooker (1985)), and on constructivism in the sociology of science (see Brown (1984), McMullin (1988, 1992). In turn these latter literatures overlap with the ocean of writing on post-modernist theory of knowledge and cognition (see Gross & Levitt (1994)). The following references relate mostly to educational constructivism, and then, with some exceptions, to articles that address epistemological and philosophical matters in science education. Even so it is not an exhaustive list, but hopefully it will be useful for teachers and researchers in the field. The author welcomes additions or omissions being brought to his attention.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号