首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   15349篇
  免费   188篇
  国内免费   10篇
教育   11229篇
科学研究   1277篇
各国文化   97篇
体育   1247篇
综合类   7篇
文化理论   97篇
信息传播   1593篇
  2021年   118篇
  2020年   191篇
  2019年   302篇
  2018年   405篇
  2017年   433篇
  2016年   389篇
  2015年   253篇
  2014年   321篇
  2013年   2893篇
  2012年   339篇
  2011年   402篇
  2010年   256篇
  2009年   257篇
  2008年   360篇
  2007年   268篇
  2006年   331篇
  2005年   223篇
  2004年   208篇
  2003年   185篇
  2002年   240篇
  2001年   333篇
  2000年   310篇
  1999年   272篇
  1998年   144篇
  1997年   177篇
  1996年   194篇
  1995年   197篇
  1994年   164篇
  1993年   176篇
  1992年   241篇
  1991年   214篇
  1990年   219篇
  1989年   253篇
  1988年   251篇
  1987年   209篇
  1986年   213篇
  1985年   222篇
  1984年   200篇
  1983年   241篇
  1982年   178篇
  1981年   169篇
  1980年   164篇
  1979年   234篇
  1978年   190篇
  1977年   168篇
  1976年   154篇
  1975年   136篇
  1974年   125篇
  1973年   130篇
  1971年   111篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 8 毫秒
121.
This paper examines 6th grade children's local conceptual development and mathematization processes as they worked a comprehensive mathematical modeling problem (creating a consumer guide for deciding the best snack chip) over several class periods. The children and their teachers were participating in a 3-year longitudinal teaching experiment in which sequences of mathematical modeling problems were implemented from the 5th grade (10 years of age) though to the 7th grade. In contrast to traditional problem solving, mathematical modeling requires children to generate and develop their own mathematical ideas and processes, and to form systems of relationships that are generalizable and reusable. Reported here is a detailed analysis of the iterative cycles of development of one group of children as they worked the problem, followed by a summary of the mathematization processes displayed by all groups. Children's critical reflections on their models are also reported. The results show how children can independently develop constructs and processes through meaningful problem solving. Children's development included creating systems for operationally defining constructs; selecting, categorizing, and ranking factors; quantifying quantitative and qualitative data; and transforming quantities.  相似文献   
122.
123.
124.
Early developmental psychologists viewed iconic representation as cognitively less complex than other forms of symbolic thought. It is therefore surprising that iconic signs are not acquired more easily than arbitrary signs by young language learners. One explanation is that children younger than 3 years have difficulty interpreting iconicity. The current study assessed hearing children's ability to interpret the meaning of iconic signs. Sixty-six 2.5- to 5-year-olds who had no previous exposure to signs were required to match iconic signs to pictures of referents. Whereas few of the 2.5-year-olds recognized the meaning of the iconic signs consistently, more than half of the 3.0-year-olds and most of 3.5-year-olds performed above chance. Thus, the ability to recognize the meaning of iconic signs gradually develops during the preschool years. Implications of these findings for sign language development, receptive signed vocabulary tests, and the development of the ability to interpret iconic symbols are discussed.  相似文献   
125.
126.
1 Introduction Duringthe past several decades ,considerable atten-tion has been given to devising methods for solvingconstrained opti mization problemsviaunconstrainedmini mization techniques and a number of researchwork in the area of nonlinear programmi…  相似文献   
127.
128.
129.
130.
The paper describes the design and use of a studio system which arose out of the need to give demonstrations of experimental procedures to practical classes of up to 150 students. Since the requirements could be specified in some detail it was possible to use novel methods of working which turned out to be very efficient and economical.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号