首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   293篇
  免费   8篇
教育   244篇
科学研究   8篇
各国文化   4篇
体育   18篇
文化理论   8篇
信息传播   19篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   4篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   21篇
  2018年   24篇
  2017年   19篇
  2016年   22篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   15篇
  2013年   77篇
  2012年   13篇
  2011年   9篇
  2010年   11篇
  2009年   5篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   7篇
  2006年   6篇
  2005年   7篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   2篇
  2002年   2篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   1篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   3篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   1篇
  1984年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1973年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
排序方式: 共有301条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
201.
We examined five-year-old-children's age stereotyping using a modified Piagetian conservation task. Children were asked if two lines of objects were the “same” after one line had been made longer (transformed). A conversational account posits that children's answers reflect assumptions about the asker's motivation for the question (Schwarz, 1996 Schwarz , N. ( 1996 ). Cognition and communication: Judgmental biases, research methods, and the logic of conversation . Mahwah , NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum . [Google Scholar]). We reasoned that when asked by a young adult (puppet Experiment 1, experimenter Experiment 2) children would assume the question is to ascertain if they have noted a perceptual change and would provide answers focused on the change (length). If older adults are assumed to be in decline because of negative age stereotyping, however, children may assume an older adult (puppet, experimenter) is seeking clarification about line equality and would, therefore, give responses focused on the similarity of the lines (number). The expectation that a focus on length or number should differ across puppet/experimenter conditions was supported in both experiments and provides behavioral evidence for children's old age stereotyping.  相似文献   
202.
The world population pyramid has changed shape. However, this does not mean that societies have changed their negative concept of old age. Our study proposes an intergenerational service-learning program with 179 university students and 101 slightly depressed elderly people. The results show that the elderly people who interacted improved in well-being. Those interacting with the young people tended to lower their stereotyped perception of themselves, while the others tended to augment it. The young people tended to moderate their stereotypes of the elderly with or without interaction.  相似文献   
203.
The authors examined whether motivational goals influenced the participation and performance of low-achieving students during collaborative problem solving with a high-achieving partner. Thirty-five pairs of 4th- and 5th-grade students were randomly assigned a set of instructions designed to induce students to adopt a learning goal or a performance goal. The following day, the students were individually given a posttest on problems similar to those worked on collaboratively. The low-achieving students given learning-goal instructions performed better on the posttest problems and perceived their partner's competence as more similar to their own than did the low-achieving students given performance-goal instructions. No differences in overall amount or level of low achievers' participation during collaborative problem solving were observed. Implications of the findings for the use of peer learning in heterogeneous classrooms are discussed.  相似文献   
204.
205.
FEMINIST PEDAGOGY THEORY: REFLECTIONS ON POWER AND AUTHORITY   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
206.
The struggle with fraction learning in kindergarten through Grade 12 in the United States is a persistent problem and one of the major stumbling blocks to succeeding in higher mathematics. Research into this problem has identified several areas where students commonly struggle with fractions. While there are many theories of fraction learning, none of the research on these theories employs samples large enough to test theories at scale or nuanced enough to demonstrate how learning unfolds over time during instructional activities based on these theories. The work reported here uses learning analytics methods with fine-grained log data from an online fraction game to unpack how splitting (i.e. partitioning a whole into equal-sized parts) impacts learning. Study 1 demonstrated that playing the game significantly improved students’ fraction understanding. In addition, a cluster analysis suggested that exploring splitting was beneficial. Study 2 replicated the learning results, and a cluster analysis showed that compared to early game play, later game play showed more optimal splitting strategies. In addition, in looking at the types of transitions that were possible between a student’s early cluster categorization and later cluster categorization, we found that some types of transitions were more beneficial for learning than others.  相似文献   
207.
This study aimed to analyse and compare the frequency of a wide range of parental discipline methods between Spain and the United States of America, according to country, parent-child gender and education level. To this end, 2,755 undergraduates, 1,416 from Spain and 1,339 from the United States, retrospectively reported the discipline that their parents had administered. Spanish parents, compared with Americans, showed a higher frequency in all types of discipline. Mothers in both countries more often applied most of the discipline methods, while the gender of the children was generally not related to the discipline received. Finally, a higher parental education level was associated with a higher rate of verbal explanation, a higher frequency of penalty tasks and with a lower use of physical punishment by the mothers. The implications of these findings are discussed based on the results of previous studies and cultural differences between the two countries.  相似文献   
208.
This paper explores socio-ecological factors and their interplay that emerge from a qualitative study and influence adolescents' physical activity and sport participation. A total of 13 boys and 7 girls active and inactive adolescents, from years 12 and 13 and different types of school (state and private), participated in semi-structured interviews. It followed a purposeful sampling selection from participants who participated in a previous larger study on physical activity levels. Findings highlight the influence of personal and social interplay, such as perceived competence, obesity and teasing from peers, and new social demands and personal preferences in adolescents' (dis)engagement in physical activity and sport. They also stand out how social and environmental factors affect their participation, such as family, boy/girlfriends, physical education and rural/urban places of residence. These factors appear to have an important impact on the construction of (in)active identities during late adolescence. Policy makers, exercise professionals and educators should be aware of these factors in order to improve health promotion strategies and social policies. Some recommendations are also indicated in the paper.  相似文献   
209.
210.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号