首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1736篇
  免费   23篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   1408篇
科学研究   65篇
各国文化   16篇
体育   64篇
文化理论   8篇
信息传播   199篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   18篇
  2020年   24篇
  2019年   29篇
  2018年   46篇
  2017年   54篇
  2016年   59篇
  2015年   37篇
  2014年   60篇
  2013年   390篇
  2012年   59篇
  2011年   59篇
  2010年   44篇
  2009年   45篇
  2008年   42篇
  2007年   56篇
  2006年   47篇
  2005年   40篇
  2004年   49篇
  2003年   39篇
  2002年   39篇
  2001年   48篇
  2000年   33篇
  1999年   28篇
  1998年   19篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   11篇
  1995年   23篇
  1994年   28篇
  1993年   23篇
  1992年   12篇
  1991年   17篇
  1990年   26篇
  1989年   16篇
  1988年   12篇
  1987年   18篇
  1986年   18篇
  1985年   15篇
  1984年   14篇
  1983年   16篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   18篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   13篇
  1978年   14篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   9篇
  1975年   4篇
  1971年   4篇
排序方式: 共有1760条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
In this study, we investigated fifth graders’ (n = 52) fall literacy, academic language, and motivation and how these skills predicted fall and spring comprehension monitoring on an eye movement task. Comprehension monitoring was defined as the identification and repair of misunderstandings when reading text. In the eye movement task, children read two sentences; the second included either a plausible or implausible word in the context of the first sentence. Stronger readers had shorter reading times overall suggesting faster processing of text. Generally fifth graders reacted to the implausible word (i.e., longer gaze duration on the implausible vs. the plausible word, which reflects lexical access). Students with stronger academic language, compared to those with weaker academic language, generally spent more time rereading the implausible target compared to the plausible target. This difference increased from fall to spring. Results support the centrality of academic language for meaning integration, setting standards of coherence, and utilizing comprehension repair strategies.  相似文献   
92.
This account of practice describes what three executive leaders in a professional association have learned about action learning and their own practices of organizational renewal. Data are approached narratively and stories are told from the perspectives of diverse educators’ experiences, agency, and expertise. Mature organizations can be revitalized and diversified through the action learning of executive leaders who integrate a new and diverse faculty body into the governance structure and decision-making process. The goal of facilitating diversity goals for established organizations has relevance for universities, schools, businesses, and other learning environments. Public conversations can strengthen the diversity-oriented outreach missions of professional organizations.  相似文献   
93.
Handedness has been studied for association with language-related disorders because of its link with language hemispheric dominance. No clear pattern has emerged, possibly because of small samples, publication bias, and heterogeneous criteria across studies. Non-right-handedness (NRH) frequency was assessed in N = 2503 cases with reading and/or language impairment and N = 4316 sex-matched controls identified from 10 distinct cohorts (age range 6–19 years old; European ethnicity) using a priori set criteria. A meta-analysis (Ncases = 1994) showed elevated NRH % in individuals with language/reading impairment compared with controls (OR = 1.21, CI = 1.06–1.39, p = .01). The association between reading/language impairments and NRH could result from shared pathways underlying brain lateralization, handedness, and cognitive functions.  相似文献   
94.
To learn language, children must map variable input to categories such as phones and words. How do children process variation and distinguish between variable pronunciations (“shoup” for soup) versus new words? The unique sensory experience of children with cochlear implants, who learn speech through their device's degraded signal, lends new insight into this question. In a mispronunciation sensitivity eyetracking task, children with implants (N = 33), and typical hearing (N = 24; 36–66 months; 36F, 19M; all non-Hispanic white), with larger vocabularies processed known words faster. But children with implants were less sensitive to mispronunciations than typical hearing controls. Thus, children of all hearing experiences use lexical knowledge to process familiar words but require detailed speech representations to process variable speech in real time.  相似文献   
95.
96.
97.
98.
99.
100.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号