首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   661篇
  免费   7篇
教育   572篇
科学研究   13篇
各国文化   8篇
体育   10篇
文化理论   3篇
信息传播   62篇
  2023年   4篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   11篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   17篇
  2017年   26篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   22篇
  2013年   173篇
  2012年   20篇
  2011年   12篇
  2010年   16篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   20篇
  2007年   15篇
  2006年   15篇
  2005年   12篇
  2004年   15篇
  2003年   11篇
  2002年   14篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   13篇
  1989年   9篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   6篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   5篇
  1977年   8篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   4篇
  1970年   3篇
排序方式: 共有668条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
162.
163.
This paper, written within a discursive perspective, explores the co-shaping of public and private discourse, and some of the circumstances under which one occasions the other, in the evolution of mathematical thinking by pairs of 13-year-olds. The discourse of six pairs of students, engaged in interpreting and graphing problem situations involving rational functions, was analyzed by means of recently developed methodological tools. The nature of the mathematics that emerged for each pair was found to be related to several factors that included the characteristics of the interpersonal object-level utterances both before and after the solution path had been generated, the degree of activity of the personal channels of the interlocutors, and the extent to which the thoughts of participants were made explicit in the public discourse. The analysis of the discursive interactions provided evidence that adolescents within novel problem situations can experience some difficulty in making their emergent thinking available to their partners in such a way that the interaction be highly mathematically productive for both of them. This revised version was published online in July 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date.  相似文献   
164.
165.
166.
167.
Following on from a previous article which examined concerns regarding the growing demand for public examination concessions on the basis of specific learning difficulties, this article further argues against the much used one‐off psychometric assessment procedure. The appropriateness is queried of a process which does not take into account such other factors as anxiety and emotional disturbance, inappropriate teaching, parental pressure, and socio‐cultural opportunity, all of which can produce a very similar pattern of failure. The nature of specific learning difficulties is also examined, plus a comment on differing assessment techniques. It was concluded that the ‘specific’ needs to be put back into an assessment of ‘specific learning difficulty’. So‐called dyslexia should be identified as the student's major special educational need and not merely as part of a more general learning problem. To do otherwise leaves the system open to abuse, and the long‐term interests of the students concerned will not be best served. The article does not necessarily reflect the views of Carolyn Blyth's employing authority.  相似文献   
168.
Toddlers' acquisition of the Novel Name–Nameless Category (N3C) principle was examined to investigate the developmental lexical principles framework and the applicability of the specificity hypothesis to relations involving lexical principles. In Study 1, we assessed the ability of 32 children between the ages of 16 and 20 months to use the N3C principle (operationally defined as the ability to fast map). As predicted, only some of the children could fast map. This finding provided evidence for a crucial tenet of the developmental lexical principles framewor: Some lexical principles are not available at the start of language acquisition. Children who had acquired the N3C principle also had significantly larger vocabularies and were significantly more likely to demonstrate 2-category exhaustive sorting abilities than children who had not acquired the principle. The 2 groups of children did not differ in either age or object permanence abilities. The 16 children who could not fast map were followed longitudinally until they attained a vocabulary spurt; at that time, their ability to fast map was retested (Study 2). Results provided a longitudinal replication of the findings of Study 1. Implications of these findings for both the developmental lexical principles framework and the specificity hypothesis are discussed.  相似文献   
169.
A method of analyzing test item responses is advocated to examine differential item functioning through distractor choices of those who answer an item incorrectly. The analysis, called Differential Distractor Functioning, uses log-linear models of a three-way contingency table to examine whether there is an interaction of population subgroup and option choice when ability is held constant. The analysis is explained and is exemplified in an analysis of the Verbal portion of a recent Scholastic Aptitude Test.  相似文献   
170.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, 1988, New Orleans.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号