全文获取类型
收费全文 | 445篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 324篇 |
科学研究 | 30篇 |
各国文化 | 5篇 |
体育 | 67篇 |
综合类 | 1篇 |
信息传播 | 20篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 19篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 111篇 |
2012年 | 14篇 |
2011年 | 7篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 8篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1924年 | 3篇 |
1923年 | 2篇 |
1921年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有447条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
361.
Kristin Lesseig Rebekah Elliott Elham Kazemi Megan Kelley-Petersen Matthew Campbell Judith Mumme Cathy Carroll 《Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education》2017,20(6):591-619
In this article, we report on Researching Mathematics Leader Learning (RMLL), a project designed to support leaders in learning how to facilitate robust opportunities for teachers’ mathematical learning. Our two-phase research design allowed us to construct a set of videocase seminars, enact the seminar design with leaders, analyze these data, refine our seminar design, and implement a second set of seminars with a new group of leaders. We drew on the noticing literature to examine leaders’ pedagogical reasoning as they discussed videocases of professional development. In this article, we demonstrate how changes in our framework for leader development and the resulting changes in the prompts and tasks shaped leader noticing in three ways: (a) accounting for the mathematical work of the facilitator and teachers in the videocase; (b) linking the mathematical work to goals for teacher learning; and (c) reasoning around the facilitator’s work in advancing those learning goals. Analysis indicates that in Phase II, leader discussions were more focused on the mathematical and pedagogical work needed to advance teacher learning. Based on our research and development work with over 70 leaders, we offer a set of design principles for leader professional development. 相似文献
362.
Ng Li Fung Lin 《Educational Studies in Mathematics》1990,21(6):509-520
Forty 6- to 7-year-old children were tested with single-step, addition/subtraction story problems with and without superfluous numerical information. Half of the children were allowed to find out the solution using objects and half were given pen and paper. Fewer problems with superfluous information than those without such information were solved accurately. Less than half of the problems were attempted with spontaneous modelling with objects and rarely were pen and paper used. In many cases, the use of objects could not facilitate solution of problems with superfluous information thus suggesting that in these cases children's failure to solve the problems could not be explained just by an increase in the cognitive demand for selective attention to, and memory of, the relevant information. 相似文献
363.
Paul Brna Mike Brayshaw Alan Bundy Mark Elsom-Cook Pat Fung Tony Dodd 《Instructional Science》1991,20(2-3):193-214
In this paper we present an overview of the advances in debugging standard Prolog programs. The analysis offered is in terms of a classification of tools that provide different degrees of activity in the debugging process. Other possible dimensions of analysis are also outlined. 相似文献
364.
S. Robertson J. Calder P. Fung A. Jones & T. O'Shea 《British journal of educational technology : journal of the Council for Educational Technology》1997,28(3):177-189
This paper reports how palmtop computers were used in an English secondary school over the course of a year. In particular, it concentrates on the perceived effectiveness of such computers in an educational setting, and on how teachers' and students' knowledge and use of content-free applications increased over that time. The benefits and potential problems with this new technology are discussed along with the consequences it has for classroom and school organisation. 相似文献
365.
James B. Hale Catherine A. Fiorello Ron Dumont John O. Willis Christopher Rackley Colin Elliott 《Psychology in the schools》2008,45(9):838-858
Concerns about the ability‐achievement discrepancy method for specific learning disability (SLD) determination led to alternative research‐based methods, such as failure to respond to intervention. Neither of these regulatory methods address the statutory SLD definition, which explicitly includes a deficit in basic psychological processes. Examining neuropsychological processing differences among typical children and children with math SLD, commonality analyses revealed that Differential Ability Scales – Second Edition (DAS‐II) predictors accounted for more achievement variance in typical children (46% to 58%) than in children with math SLD (33% to 50%), with substantial loss of predictive validity when General Conceptual Ability was used instead of subcomponent scores. Results suggest that differences in typical predictor‐outcome relationships may provide a foundation for developing specific cognitive and academic interventions for children with math SLD. © 2008 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 相似文献
366.
OBJECTIVE: The present study tests a model linking attachment, childhood sexual abuse (CSA), and adult psychological functioning. It expands on previous work by assessing the degree to which attachment security moderates the relationship between a history of child sexual abuse and trauma-related symptoms in college females. METHOD: Self-reports of attachment, childhood sexual abuse, and adult psychological functioning were obtained from 324 female undergraduate students attending a Southeastern U.S. university. Separate analyses were conducted examining the potential moderating role for close-adult, parent-child, and peer attachment styles. RESULTS: In this sample, 37.7% of participants reported sexually abusive experiences prior to age 16. History of child sexual abuse was consistently associated with higher levels of trauma-related symptoms and lower levels of attachment security in close-adult, parent-child, and peer relations. Additionally, attachment security was consistently associated with trauma-related symptoms. Close-adult, parent-child, and peer attachment differentially moderated trauma-related symptoms. Specifically, in peer relationships, the strength of the relationships between attachment measures and trauma symptoms were greater for CSA survivors than for non-abused participants. The opposite pattern of results was found for attachment in parental and close-adult relationships. CONCLUSION: Results suggest that attachment security in peer and parent relationships protects against the negative effects of CSA, while only weak, marginally significant protective effects were observed for close-adult relationships. Only modest support was found for the conceptualization of attachment as a moderator of the relationship between CSA and trauma-related symptoms. However, the results suggest that attachment security at least partially protects against negative CSA outcomes. 相似文献
367.
Diane Sookyoung Lee Tina G. Dang Jennifer Ulibas-Pascual Kimberly A. Gordon Biddle Brian Heller de Leon Deborah Elliott Josiah Gorter 《The Urban Review》2017,49(5):707-728
The purpose of this study was to determine the effectiveness of a local afterschool program in helping underserved elementary school students improve their reading achievement. The study followed a cohort of students with 28 participants who were low-income, ethnic minority children in the 1st–6th grades between the ages of 6 and 12. Data on students’ developmental assets and school progress were collected at two different time points. Overall, students showed increased perceptions of efficacy about having the ability and skills to do well on a task and getting good grades in school, as well as improvements in reading achievement over time. While changes in homework completion predicted changes in reading achievement, changes in efficacy beliefs and program exposure did not. Results suggest that teacher perceptions of homework completion may be an important pathway through which afterschool programs can help narrow the reading achievement gap experienced by ethnic minority students from disadvantaged backgrounds. 相似文献
368.
Tracy Packiam Alloway Susan Elizabeth Gathercole Hannah Kirkwood Julian Elliott 《Child development》2009,80(2):606-621
This study explored the cognitive and behavioral profiles of children with working memory impairments. In an initial screening of 3,189 five- to eleven-year-olds, 308 were identified as having very low working memory scores. Cognitive skills (IQ, vocabulary, reading, and math), classroom behavior, and self-esteem were assessed. The majority of the children struggled in the learning measures and verbal ability. They also obtained atypically high ratings of cognitive problems/inattentive symptoms and were judged to have short attention spans, high levels of distractibility, problems in monitoring the quality of their work, and difficulties in generating new solutions to problems. These data provide rich new information on the cognitive and behavioral profiles that characterize children with low working memory. 相似文献
369.
Kimberley Alkins Linda Banks-Santilli Paula Elliott Nicole Guttenberg Mieko Kamii 《Equity & Excellence in Education》2013,46(1):65-73
On November 16, 1972, U. S. District Judge John H. Pratt ruled in Adams v. Richardson. Following is the complete text of the decision. 相似文献
370.
The ability to make inferences about what one’s peers know is critical for social interaction and communication. Three experiments (n = 309) examined the curse of knowledge, the tendency to be biased by one’s knowledge when reasoning about others’ knowledge, in children’s estimates of their peers’ knowledge. Four- to 7-year-olds were taught the answers to factual questions and estimated how many peers would know the answers. When children learned familiar answers, they showed a curse of knowledge in their peer estimates. But, when children learned unfamiliar answers to the same questions, they did not show a curse of knowledge. These data shed light on the mechanisms underlying perspective taking, supporting a fluency misattribution account of the curse of knowledge. 相似文献