全文获取类型
收费全文 | 787篇 |
免费 | 21篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 622篇 |
科学研究 | 62篇 |
各国文化 | 12篇 |
体育 | 64篇 |
综合类 | 3篇 |
文化理论 | 4篇 |
信息传播 | 44篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 34篇 |
2019年 | 55篇 |
2018年 | 40篇 |
2017年 | 46篇 |
2016年 | 44篇 |
2015年 | 30篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 128篇 |
2012年 | 38篇 |
2011年 | 30篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 14篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1960年 | 2篇 |
1958年 | 2篇 |
1924年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有811条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Jackie Masterson Veronica Laxon Emma Carnegie Sheila Wright Janice Horslen 《Journal of Research in Reading》2005,28(2):183-201
Previous research has established that the degree of ‘wordlikeness’ of nonwords affects young children's nonword repetition performance. Experiment 1 examined the possibility that output processes are responsible for the wordlikeness effect by using a probed recall procedure. Wordlikeness was defined in terms of phonological neighbourhood density, although this measure was found to be related to the traditional measure of wordlikeness involving adult ratings. A significant effect of number of phonological neighbours/wordlikeness was observed in favour of nonwords with many neighbours. In Experiments 2 and 3 the wordlikeness effect was qualified by a significant interaction with nonword repetition ability. Children with poorer repetition ability were affected by number of neighbours/wordlikeness, while children with better repetition ability were not. Children with poorer repetition ability were significantly poorer than the better repeaters with nonwords with few neighbours. The results were interpreted in terms of theories of phonological development that suggest progressive segmentation of lexical representations. In Experiment 4 the relationship of children's nonword repetition ability to phonemic discrimination ability was investigated. The results demonstrated that children with better nonword repetition ability had superior phonemic discrimination performance than children with poorer nonword repetition ability. 相似文献
72.
Same beginnings, different stories: a comparison of American and Chinese children's narratives 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
This study examined social, emotional, and cognitive characteristics of American and Chinese children's narratives. Twenty-four American and 26 Chinese 6-year-old children participated. Each child was interviewed individually twice with a 1-week delay interval. During the two interviews, children were asked to tell 11 stories prompted by pictures and standard verbal leads and to recount 7 emotional memories. Content analyses were performed on children's stories and memories. In line with predictions, findings indicated that compared with American children, Chinese children showed greater orientation toward social engagement, greater concern with moral correctness, greater concern with authority, a less autonomous orientation, more expressions of emotions, and more situational details in both their stories and memories. A few gender differences were found. Findings are discussed in terms of different value systems and early socialization practices in these two cultures. 相似文献
73.
74.
75.
76.
The study examined whether schools for the deaf were providing services to assist parents in communicating with their children about sexuality (including sexual signs) and whether parents were involved in the sexuality education curriculum within their child's school. The Sexuality Curriculum Questionnaire for Educators of Students Who Are Deaf (Getch & Gabriel, 1998) was completed by 71 educators teaching sexuality curricula in schools for the deaf across the United States. Results indicated that parents were more likely to be involved in approval and development of their children's sexuality education than to receive assistance with sexuality education from the schools. Although the level of parental participation in curriculum development and approval is encouraging, the number of parents actually participating in curriculum development and approval remains low. 相似文献
77.
Trade in Australian education services has expanded rapidly over recent years. The sector is the third largest exporter of
Australian services. In 2001–2002, exports of education were about $A 4.2 billion. Government assistance to the sector includes
export market development, regulation of education standards, and funding education activities; university research and development,
for example. This paper examines the case for further government intervention in the export of education, and the appropriate
forms of assistance if further government intervention is justified. The paper predominately focuses on assisting higher education
exports because this activity dominates education exports. 相似文献
78.
Claire M. A. Haworth Emma L. Meaburn Nicole Harlaar Robert Plomin 《Mind, Brain, and Education》2007,1(4):173-180
ABSTRACT— Twin-study research suggests that many (but not all) of the same genes contribute to genetic influence on diverse learning abilities and disabilities, a hypothesis called generalist genes . This generalist genes hypothesis was tested using a set of 10 DNA markers (single nucleotide polymorphisms [SNPs]) found to be associated with early reading ability in a study of 4,258 7-year-old children that screened 100,000 SNPs. Using the same sample, we show that this early reading SNP set also correlates with other aspects of literacy, components of mathematics, and more general cognitive abilities. These results provide support for the generalist genes hypothesis. Although the effect size of the current SNP set is small, such SNP sets could eventually be used to predict genetic risk for learning disabilities as well as to prescribe genetically tailored intervention and prevention programs. 相似文献
79.
OBJECTIVE: Our studies compared individuals at high- and low-risk for child physical abuse on measures of social information processing. METHOD: Two studies were conducted using similar methods. Twenty-eight childless women in Study 1 and 36 mothers in Study 2 read vignettes of parent-child interactions in which the child's level of compliance was difficult to interpret. Participants were asked a series of questions about the child's behavior and their own reactions. RESULTS: Accuracy and bias in identifying compliant behavior were assessed using a signal detection paradigm. In both samples, high- and low-risk participants did not differ in their overall accuracy in identifying children's behaviors. However, they used different evaluation standards such that high-risk participants were biased toward seeing more noncompliance and low-risk participants were biased toward seeing more compliance. High- and low-risk participants also made different types of errors in interpreting children's behavior. Low-risk participants were more likely to misinterpret noncompliant behavior as compliant, and there was a trend for high-risk participants to not perceive compliant behavior when it occurred. There were no differences in reported disciplinary responses in either study and the results for affective reactions were mixed. CONCLUSIONS: Specific differences in social information processing between high- and low-risk individuals replicated across samples, suggesting a reliable association between evaluation standards and risk of child physical abuse. However, the absence of differences in reported discipline and inconsistent findings on affective reactions indicate the need to identify the mechanism through which cognition influences parenting behavior. 相似文献
80.