首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   582篇
  免费   8篇
教育   396篇
科学研究   51篇
各国文化   11篇
体育   33篇
文化理论   9篇
信息传播   90篇
  2021年   4篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   14篇
  2017年   13篇
  2016年   18篇
  2015年   7篇
  2014年   18篇
  2013年   150篇
  2012年   11篇
  2011年   13篇
  2010年   13篇
  2009年   6篇
  2008年   10篇
  2007年   5篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   11篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   18篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   3篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   5篇
  1989年   8篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   12篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   4篇
  1979年   6篇
  1977年   10篇
  1976年   4篇
  1974年   3篇
  1972年   3篇
  1970年   4篇
  1968年   5篇
  1966年   3篇
  1923年   4篇
  1922年   3篇
  1920年   5篇
  1919年   3篇
排序方式: 共有590条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
This study investigated plant and animal photographs in elementary science textbooks to discern whether there were disparities in the number of plant and animal photographs or in how those photographs were labelled. We examined the Life Science sections of two nationally syndicated (USA) textbook series. For each text, we identified the photographs with plant and/or animal content and evaluated them for two features: (1) the subject of the photograph, and (2) the specificity of the label (name) provided. We found that photographs with animal subjects were more numerous than those with plant subjects; they also represented a greater diversity of animals and had a higher instance of repetition than did plant photographs. We also found a significant naming disparity: animal photographs were three times more likely to be provided with a specific label (common name) than were plant photographs. Not only were plant photographs less likely to be provided with a specific name for the plant (e.g. orchid or dandelion), but also they were commonly identified only by the name for a plant part (such as flower or leaf) or life‐form (e.g. tree or shrub). To address the disparity revealed by this study, and to encourage student interest in and knowledge about plants, we recommend that educators go beyond textbooks to expose students to a diversity of named plants, and present plants as distinct organisms rather than as a collection of parts.  相似文献   
102.
A questionnaire was filled out by 108 graduates of a B.A. program in psychology. The subjects provided biographical data and information about credits in psychology, GPA, fields of interest, reasons for majoring in psychology, psychology-related employment during undergraduate study, plans for graduate study, occupational use of psychology after graduation, occupational and/or personal value of the B.A. program, value of faculty help and advisement, and outstanding characteristics of helpful faculty. Analyses of the data led to 11 conclusions, some of which confirm the findings of a previous study and others of which provide new information about the uniform as well as differential effects of the undergraduate curriculum.  相似文献   
103.
This essay argues for an enlarged conceptualization of deception. A multidimensional scaling of similarity judgments of forty‐six deception‐related terms identified a number of aspects underlying the construct. They included evaluation, detectability, and premeditation. The elicited terms were subjected to a hierarchical cluster analysis, which revealed six families of deception: fictions, playings, lies, crimes, masks, and unlies. Implications of these results were explored.  相似文献   
104.
Most people state that they would be willing to be organ donors, however only a small percentage of the population has actually signed an organ donor card. These studies focused on persuasive messages that encourage people to sign organ donor cards. In the first study, people reported their attitudes and knowledge involving organ donation. Results indicated that donor card signing was related to overall knowledge about donation. When only considering those people with positive attitudes toward organ donation, their overall knowledge about donation explained their willingness to sign organ donor cards. In the second study, people read a message involving organ donation before they were asked to sign an organ donor card. The content of the message (i.e., narrative vs. statistics) and the affect of the message (i.e., humorous vs. sad) were manipulated. Results indicated that narrative messages were more effective than statistical messages. Additionally, humorous messages were more effective than sad messages.  相似文献   
105.
106.
107.
108.
Going back at least to Duhem, there is a tradition of thinkingthat crucial experiments are impossible in science. I analyseDuhem's arguments and show that they are based on the excessivelystrong assumption that only deductive reasoning is permissiblein experimental science. This opens the possibility that someprinciple of inductive inference could provide a sufficientreason for preferring one among a group of hypotheses on thebasis of an appropriately controlled experiment. To be sure,there are analogues to Duhem's problems that pertain to inductiveinference. Using a famous experiment from the history of molecularbiology as an example, I show that an experimentalist versionof inference to the best explanation (IBE) does a better jobin handling these problems than other accounts of scientificinference. Furthermore, I introduce a concept of experimentalmechanism and show that it can guide inferences from data withinan IBE-based framework for induction.
  1. Introduction
  2. Duhem onthe Logic of Crucial Experiments
  3. ‘The Most BeautifulExperiment in Biology’
  4. Why Not Simple Elimination?
  5. SevereTesting
  6. An Experimentalist Version of IBE
    6.1 Physiologicaland experimentalmechanisms
    6.2 Explaining the data
    6.3IBE and the problemof untested auxiliaries
    6.4 IBE-turtlesall the way down
  7. Van Fraassen's ‘Bad Lot’ Argument
  8. IBE and Bayesianism
  9. Conclusions
  相似文献   
109.
International openness is essential to science in small modern countries. The modernization of Swiss science and economy in the nineteenth century was promoted largely by foreigners: newly founded Swiss universities were staffed to a large extent by foreign professors, but students also came from abroad. Up to the middle of this century, the development of Swiss universities was marked by distinct successive phases of inviting in or shutting out international influence, depending mainly on changes in the political context. The international landscape of knowledge and science since the 1950s has undergone dynamic change. University structures were installed as a reaction to this dynamism. Patterns of international research orientation emerging in different universities and in the disciplinaryfields of business administration, chemistry and history are discussedin a comparative perspective.  相似文献   
110.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号