全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2349篇 |
免费 | 22篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 1873篇 |
科学研究 | 46篇 |
各国文化 | 41篇 |
体育 | 121篇 |
综合类 | 1篇 |
文化理论 | 8篇 |
信息传播 | 281篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 26篇 |
2020年 | 49篇 |
2019年 | 71篇 |
2018年 | 107篇 |
2017年 | 117篇 |
2016年 | 103篇 |
2015年 | 60篇 |
2014年 | 90篇 |
2013年 | 578篇 |
2012年 | 82篇 |
2011年 | 69篇 |
2010年 | 74篇 |
2009年 | 60篇 |
2008年 | 72篇 |
2007年 | 57篇 |
2006年 | 41篇 |
2005年 | 63篇 |
2004年 | 55篇 |
2003年 | 49篇 |
2002年 | 33篇 |
2001年 | 26篇 |
2000年 | 23篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有2371条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Cluster analysis was used to identify groups defined by the patterning of fathers' and mothers' sources of knowledge about adolescents' experiences in a sample of 179 families with adolescents (M = 16.5 years). Three clusters emerged for fathers (relational, relies on spouse, relies on others) and mothers (relational, questioners, relies on others). Cluster membership was associated with socioeconomic status, work hours, personal characteristics, and parent-child relationship quality. Longitudinal path analyses revealed that knowledge sources predicted levels of knowledge, which in turn predicted risky behavior 1 year later, indirect paths that were more consistent for fathers than for mothers. Although direct associations between sources of knowledge and subsequent risky behavior were scant, when fathers relied on spouses, youth engaged in less risky behavior. 相似文献
12.
The motivation for this research was to understand under what conditions supply chain cooperation might be feasible and under what circumstances it is not. Whereas previous research focuses on dyads in a supply chain, our research seeks to examine a possible factor that can explain why truly cooperative supply chains involving more than two firms are a rarity among independent firms. This research extends the use of game theory to demonstrate the potential impact of a multiple‐tier supply chain agreement. 相似文献
13.
14.
15.
Jesse R. Cukierkorn Frances A. Karnes Sandra J. Manning Heather Houston Kevin Besnoy 《Roeper Review》2013,35(4):271-276
Highlighting the unique educational needs of gifted and high ability preschoolers, this article guides the reader to consider the characteristics of young gifted children along with appropriate assessment practices in planning educational programming. A triarchic approach to programs and services for gifted preschoolers is outlined with major emphases on an integrated curriculum, ongoing evaluation, and parental involvement. Specific strategies including internet resources are described to guide practitioners in providing optimal learning environments for this distinctive student population. 相似文献
16.
Heather B. Taylor Jason L. Anthony Rachel Aghara Karen E. Smith Susan H. Landry 《Early education and development》2008,19(1):188-207
This longitudinal study evaluated the extent to which maternal responsiveness across early childhood and children's cognitive skills predicted children's 8-year decoding and reading comprehension skills for children who varied in biological risk (term, n = 83; preterm, n = 155). Patterns of maternal responsiveness during infancy (6, 12, and 24 months) and preschool (3 and 4 years) revealed 4 maternal clusters that varied in consistency and level of maternal responsiveness. Although not predictive of decoding skills, the interaction between children's 4-year cognitive ability and maternal responsiveness cluster predicted children's reading comprehension skills at 8 years of age, regardless of risk. Although consistently high levels of maternal responsive parenting across early childhood related to literacy outcomes for all children in the study, responsive parenting had a stronger relation to later reading comprehension skills for children with lower cognitive abilities, particularly when mothers demonstrated high responsiveness in children's infancy. 相似文献
17.
Ann Rippin Charles Booth Stuart Bowie Judith Jordan 《Teaching in Higher Education》2013,18(4):429-441
The case method is a definitive and foundational technology in business education. It was first developed as a vocational training tool equipping students for future employment. In this conception, the organisation was seen as a machine, and managers as the engineers who would maintain it through planned interventions. The case method allowed aspiring managers to practise intervention skills in a safe environment. Since the origins of the case method in US business schools before the First World War, conceptions of organisations have moved on. They are no longer seen as machine-like, but as complex, ambiguous, and protean. The 'wicked' problems that potential managers will face in an uncertain world require the development of critical thinking and sensemaking abilities. While the traditional approach to the case continues, its use as a vehicle to explore and manage complexity and ambiguity is emerging, although this is facing resistance from some students and staff. 相似文献
18.
ABSTRACT Cities in the United States and across the world have experienced gentrification at the same time as school choice policies have become more popular. This research examines the relationship between gentrification and charter schooling, seeking to understand how together they affect demographic composition of schools across Washington, DC. This study uses geographic information systems (GIS) mapping and statistical techniques to show that gentrified neighborhoods are more likely to have charter schools. Additionally, the demographic compositions of charter schools and traditional public schools differ depending on the gentrification classification of the census tract in which the schools are located. While a handful of diverse charter schools exist in gentrified neighborhoods and some diverse public schools exist in traditionally affluent neighborhoods, schools in Washington, DC remain racially and economically isolated overall. 相似文献
19.
Anita Munnelly Georgina Martin Charlotte Dack Ann Zedginidze Louise McHugh 《Learning & behavior》2014,42(3):270-280
Previous studies have found that social exclusion can cause distress to those excluded. One method used to study social exclusion is through a virtual ball-toss game known as Cyberball. In this game, participants may be excluded from or included in the ball-toss game and typically report lower feelings of self-esteem, control, belonging, and meaningful existence following exclusion. Experiments 1 and 2 sought to explore the transfer of feelings of exclusion and inclusion through stimulus equivalence classes. In both experiments, participants were trained to form two three-member equivalence classes (e.g., A1–B1, B1–C1; A2–B2, B2–C2) and were tested with novel stimulus combinations (A1–C1, C1–A1, A2–C2, C2–A2). Thereafter, participants were exposed to the Cyberball exclusion and inclusion games. In these games, one stimulus (C1) from one equivalence class was assigned as the Cyberball inclusion game name, whereas one stimulus (C2) from the other equivalence class was assigned as the Cyberball exclusion game name. In Experiment 2, participants were only exposed to the Cyberball exclusion game. During a subsequent transfer test, participants were asked to rate how included in or excluded from they thought they would be in other online games, corresponding to members of both equivalence classes. Participant reported that they felt they would be excluded from online games if the games were members of the same equivalence class as C2. In contrast, participants reported that they felt they would be included in online games if the games were members of the same equivalence class as C1. Results indicated the transfer of feelings of inclusion (Experiment 1) and feelings of exclusion (Experiments 1 and 2) through equivalence classes. 相似文献
20.
In 2 experiments we asked whether training in multiple contexts could eliminate context-dependent memory retrieval at 6 months as it does for adults. We found that 24-hour retention was disrupted when infants were trained in one context and tested in another but not when they were trained in multiple contexts prior to testing in a novel context (Experiment 1). After a long delay, however, training in multiple contexts did not facilitate memory retrieval in a novel context: An otherwise effective retrieval cue (the training mobile) did not alleviate forgetting 3 weeks later when it was presented in a novel context (Experiment 2). These findings demonstrate that multiple learning contexts can override the debilitating effects of an altered context on memory retrieval at 6 months, but only over the short term. The resistance of older memories to retrieval in novel contexts after long periods of disuse may be adaptive, insuring that potentially inappropriate or irrelevant memories will not be expressed. 相似文献