全文获取类型
收费全文 | 324篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 220篇 |
科学研究 | 22篇 |
各国文化 | 4篇 |
体育 | 25篇 |
文化理论 | 5篇 |
信息传播 | 48篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 4篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 104篇 |
2012年 | 6篇 |
2011年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 3篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1933年 | 1篇 |
1925年 | 1篇 |
1922年 | 1篇 |
1920年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有324条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
Hugh W. Catts Autumn McIlraith Mindy Sittner Bridges Diane Corcoran Nielsen 《Reading and writing》2017,30(3):613-629
Participants were administered multiple measures of phonological awareness, oral language, and rapid automatized naming at the beginning of kindergarten and multiple measures of word reading at the end of second grade. A structural equation model was fit to the data and latent scores were used to identify children with a deficit in phonological awareness alone or in combination with other kindergarten deficits. Children with a deficit in phonological awareness in kindergarten were found to be five times more likely to have dyslexia in second grade than children without such a deficit. This risk ratio substantially increased with the addition of deficits in both oral language and rapid naming. Whereas children with one or more kindergarten deficits were at heighten risk for dyslexia, some of these children were found to be adequate or better readers. These results are discussed within a multifactorial model of dyslexia that includes both risk and protective factors. 相似文献
112.
Andrea Bunting Libby Robin Suzanne Uniacke Nicolas Rasmussen Graham Holland Marilys Guillemin Phillip Catton Sverre Myhra Rachel A. Ankeny David Oldroyd Ivan Crozier Peter Anstey Cathy Legg Jan Edward Garrett David Philip Miller John Worrall Andy Pickering Hugh Lacey S. S. Schweber 《Metascience》1999,8(1):125-195
113.
114.
This research sought to answer two questions: (1) What are Utah junior and senior high school students' preferences and choices regarding science subjects? (2) Could preferences and choices be related to the type of school, age or gender? Two thousand students from grades six through twelve participated in this study. Findings show that zoology and human anatomy and physiology were most preferred. Ecology was least prefered. Topics in the physical sciences were also low. There was a trend among girls to prefer natural sciences such as botany while boys tended to prefer the physical sciences. Generally, students' choices were limited to those subjects presently taught in the formal school curriculum. They appeared unaware of the many science related subjects outside the texts or the approved course of study. 相似文献
115.
Thomas Hugh Feeley Shin-Il Moon Ryan S. Kozey Anne Solbu Slowe 《Journal of Applied Communication Research》2013,41(2):167-188
An Erosion Model (EM) of employee turnover is introduced to explain the previously observed negative relationship between network centrality and employee turnover. The EM hypothesizes that social support moderates the centrality–turnover relation as those more active in the organization's social network experience less job strain due to esteem provided by work peers. Three EM hypotheses were supported using meta-analytic procedures with significant relations identified between centrality and turnover (k=5, r=.29), social support and centrality (k=7, r=.23), and social support and turnover intentions (k=17, r=.35). Future research for EM expansion and practical applications are proposed. 相似文献
116.
117.
One hundred four students watched a videotape of six communicators (three liars and three truthtellers) and were asked to judge the veracity of the strangers’ communication. In addition to judging deception, students reported which cites guided their veridical judgments. To limit their available cognitive capacity one‐half of the participants were given an additional cognitive task to complete (counting backwards from a number by 7) while viewing the videotapes. Results indicated that lie detectors with limited cognitive capacity reported using more vocal cues and fewer verbal cues than lie detectors whose cognitive capacity was not manipulated. Lie detectors with limited capacity were more accurate at catching liars and reported fewer truthful judgments than lie detectors in the high capacity condition. Overall, lie detectors reported using primarily demeanor cues and visual cues to distinguish truths from lies. Results also indicated that lie detectors’ beliefs about deception differed as a function of the communicators’ veracity. Specifically, when judging truthful communicators, lie detectors reported using fewer vocal cues and demeanor cues while relying on a greater number of verbal and visual cues as compared to lie detectors judging deceptive communicators. 相似文献
118.
Hugh Finsten 《Government Information Quarterly》1985,2(1):13-25
The extent of government involvement in public activity and the highly technical aspects of the issues arising in our legislatures, points to the need for expert assistance for lawmakers. In Canada, this need was addressed at the federal level by the establishment of a research service as a unit of the Parliamentary Library in 1965. This article gives an insight into the structure and variety of services provided by the 50 professionals of the research unit and the new directions it is taking in addressing the challenges in its work for Canada's two Houses of Parliament. 相似文献
119.
120.
Morton R H 《Journal of sports sciences》2006,24(5):495-499
This study evaluates home advantages both for national (Super 12) and international (Tri-nations) rugby union teams from South Africa, Australia and New Zealand, over the five-year period 2000 - 2004 using linear modelling. These home advantages are examined for statistical and practical significance, for variability between teams, for stability over time and for inter-correlation. These data reveal that the overall home advantage in elite rugby union has a mean of +6.7 points, and that this changes little from year to year. Closer scrutiny nevertheless reveals a high degree of variability. Different teams can and do have different home advantages, which ranges from a low of -0.7 to a high of +28.3 points in any one year. Furthermore, some team home advantages change up or down from one year to the next, by as much as -36.5 to +31.4 points at the extremes. There is no evidence that the stronger teams have the higher home advantages, or that a high home advantage leads to a superior finishing position in the competition. 相似文献