首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   319篇
  免费   4篇
教育   220篇
科学研究   21篇
各国文化   4篇
体育   25篇
文化理论   5篇
信息传播   48篇
  2020年   5篇
  2019年   3篇
  2018年   4篇
  2017年   6篇
  2016年   12篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   103篇
  2012年   6篇
  2011年   9篇
  2010年   6篇
  2009年   8篇
  2008年   3篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   10篇
  2005年   4篇
  2003年   4篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   5篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   8篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   3篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   3篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   3篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   2篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   3篇
  1966年   2篇
  1933年   1篇
  1925年   1篇
  1922年   1篇
  1920年   1篇
排序方式: 共有323条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
121.
休·唐士力出生于伦敦,受教育于英国,他最先学习拉丁和希腊“传统” (或者说死)语言,继而改学“现代”(或者说活)语言:法语、德语、西班牙语。其大部分的职业生涯都在位于法国的一家国际宇航公司的商务部度过,但他始终对语言、语言结构和语言发展保持了浓厚的兴趣。他因为工作原因第一次去中国是在1990年。在后来的12年里,他以每年10次的频率频繁出  相似文献   
122.
123.
This article explores how pupils and teachers in an 11–16 mixed secondary school in an area of urban disadvantage in the UK experience pupil voice. It used visual methods to unpick some of the ways in which official and unofficial discourses of pupil voice, engagement, discipline and inclusion were played out in this school. A typology of pupils, based on analysis of school policy documentation was produced. Whilst these ‘types’ were expressed through pupil scrapbooks and interviews, they were not found to be related to individual pupils in the way that the school policy documentation suggests. Adults respond to pupil voice differently depending on how it is framed—the ‘types’ create discursive practices that determine the things that can be said, by whom and in what way. The visual methods used are reviewed here in the light of findings and are found to be useful in eliciting a range of pupil voices.  相似文献   
124.
125.
Knowledge as a valuable asset of organizations is increasingly incorporated into thinking about strategy. Studies of knowledge management (KM) suggest that executives engaged in decision making often have a slender understanding of the strategic significance of knowledge. When addressing the challenge of explicating and designing a knowledge strategy, logics of codification and personalization have been differentiated and commended. The paper draws upon evidence from four case studies to identify factors that shape the evolving contexts of knowledge strategies. It is in these contexts that the challenge of continuously reviewing and revising the mix of codifying and personalizing aspects of strategic KM is practically accomplished. The cases are analysed with reference to external competition, leadership, organizational politics, culture and technology as a basis for advancing a more dynamic framework for the analysis of knowledge strategies.  相似文献   
126.
This study investigated the role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Measures of response time in motor, visual, lexical, grammatical, and phonological tasks were administered to 279 children in third grade. Measures of rapid object naming, phonological awareness, and reading achievement were given in second and fourth grades. Reading group comparisons indicated that poor readers were proportionally slower than good readers across response time measures and on the rapid object naming task. These results suggest that some poor readers have a general deficit in speed of processing and that their problems in rapid object naming are in part a reflection of this deficit. Hierarchical regression analyses further showed that when considered along with IQ and phonological awareness, speed of processing explained unique variance in reading achievement. This finding suggests that a speed of processing deficit may be an "extraphonological" factor in some reading disabilities.  相似文献   
127.
128.
129.
An attempt was made to train 9 homing pigeons to respond to the presence or absence of bar magnets by turning either left or right after flying the length of a 20-ft outdoor flight cage. During initial training, color cues were placed in front of feeding stations on the left and right sides of the cage. The color cues were paired with magnetic cues by attaching either bar magnets or brass bars to the backs of the birds. The color cues were then deleted, leaving only the magnetic cues. Each pigeon received about 300 trials of color training followed by about 200 trials of magnet testing. When only magnetic cues remained, none of the pigeons were able to choose the correct feeder at greater than chance levels of probability.  相似文献   
130.
The present study investigated the use of the Reading Component Model to subgroup poor readers. A large sample of poor readers was identified in second grade and subgrouped on the basis of relative strengths and weaknesses in word recognition and listening comprehension. Although homogeneous subgroups were not identified, poor readers could be classified into four subgroups that differed significantly in reading-related abilities. Further analyses showed that poor readers' strengths and weaknesses in listening comprehension, and to a lesser extent in word recognition, were foreshadowed by their abilities on related kindergarten measures. Follow-up testing in the fourth grade indicated that poor readers' individual differences in word recognition and listening comprehension were consistent and that subgroups were moderately stable. The implications of these results for the assessment and remediation of reading disabilities are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号