首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   11232篇
  免费   152篇
  国内免费   17篇
教育   8262篇
科学研究   794篇
各国文化   201篇
体育   886篇
综合类   5篇
文化理论   156篇
信息传播   1097篇
  2022年   47篇
  2021年   109篇
  2020年   216篇
  2019年   308篇
  2018年   355篇
  2017年   391篇
  2016年   398篇
  2015年   248篇
  2014年   301篇
  2013年   2533篇
  2012年   273篇
  2011年   306篇
  2010年   256篇
  2009年   258篇
  2008年   308篇
  2007年   276篇
  2006年   236篇
  2005年   266篇
  2004年   233篇
  2003年   223篇
  2002年   227篇
  2001年   176篇
  2000年   176篇
  1999年   144篇
  1998年   112篇
  1997年   146篇
  1996年   141篇
  1995年   114篇
  1994年   129篇
  1993年   108篇
  1992年   131篇
  1991年   131篇
  1990年   120篇
  1989年   101篇
  1988年   98篇
  1987年   103篇
  1986年   103篇
  1985年   121篇
  1984年   114篇
  1983年   118篇
  1982年   96篇
  1981年   91篇
  1980年   77篇
  1979年   99篇
  1978年   108篇
  1977年   79篇
  1976年   61篇
  1975年   59篇
  1974年   40篇
  1973年   42篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
61.
In collaborative problem solving, children produce and evaluate arguments for proposals. We investigated whether 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 192) can produce and evaluate arguments against those arguments (i.e., counter-arguments). In Study 1, each child within a peer dyad was privately given a reason to prefer one over another solution to a task. One child, however, was given further information that would refute the reasoning of their partner. Five-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, identified and produced valid and relevant counter-arguments. In Study 2, 3-year-olds were given discourse training (discourse that contrasted valid and invalid counter-arguments) and then given the same problem-solving tasks. After training, 3-year-olds could also identify and produce valid and relevant counter-arguments. Thus, participating in discourse about reasons facilitates children’s counter-argumentation.  相似文献   
62.
63.
64.
65.
One of the most controversial issues in special education over the last 40 years has been the extent to which students with learning disabilities (LD) should be educated in general education classrooms. Recent mandates in federal law requiring that all students with disabilities have access to the general education curriculum and make adequate yearly progress relative to this content have intensified this debate. In this article, a brief summary of research regarding the nature of instruction that produces significantly improved educational outcomes for students with LD is provided. This is followed by a review of research related to the delivery of this high‐quality instruction in inclusive, general education classrooms and in resource settings. We conclude that this research provides the foundation for reconsidering full inclusion and how services are delivered for elementary students with LD.  相似文献   
66.
Research suggests that spatial ability may predict success in complex disciplines including anatomy, where mastery requires a firm understanding of the intricate relationships occurring along the course of veins, arteries, and nerves, as they traverse through and around bones, muscles, and organs. Debate exists on the malleability of spatial ability, and some suggest that spatial ability can be enhanced through training. It is hypothesized that spatial ability can be trained in low-performing individuals through visual guidance. To address this, training was completed through a visual guidance protocol. This protocol was based on eye-movement patterns of high-performing individuals, collected via eye-tracking as they completed an Electronic Mental Rotations Test (EMRT). The effects of guidance were evaluated using 33 individuals with low mental rotation ability, in a counterbalanced crossover design. Individuals were placed in one of two treatment groups (late or early guidance) and completed both a guided, and an unguided EMRT. A third group (no guidance/control) completed two unguided EMRTs. All groups demonstrated an increase in EMRT scores on their second test (P < 0.001); however, an interaction was observed between treatment and test iteration (P = 0.024). The effect of guidance on scores was contingent on when the guidance was applied. When guidance was applied early, scores were significantly greater than expected (P = 0.028). These findings suggest that by guiding individuals with low mental rotation ability “where” to look early in training, better search approaches may be adopted, yielding improvements in spatial reasoning scores. It is proposed that visual guidance may be applied in spatial fields, such as STEMM (science, technology, engineering, mathematics and medicine), surgery, and anatomy to improve student's interpretation of visual content. Anat Sci Educ. © 2018 American Association of Anatomists.  相似文献   
67.
68.
69.
70.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号