全文获取类型
收费全文 | 11726篇 |
免费 | 60篇 |
国内免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 7603篇 |
科学研究 | 2119篇 |
各国文化 | 86篇 |
体育 | 797篇 |
综合类 | 5篇 |
文化理论 | 73篇 |
信息传播 | 1113篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 110篇 |
2020年 | 164篇 |
2019年 | 246篇 |
2018年 | 302篇 |
2017年 | 277篇 |
2016年 | 221篇 |
2015年 | 172篇 |
2014年 | 219篇 |
2013年 | 2041篇 |
2012年 | 233篇 |
2011年 | 213篇 |
2010年 | 187篇 |
2009年 | 176篇 |
2008年 | 175篇 |
2007年 | 194篇 |
2006年 | 158篇 |
2005年 | 158篇 |
2004年 | 148篇 |
2003年 | 142篇 |
2002年 | 138篇 |
2001年 | 174篇 |
2000年 | 193篇 |
1999年 | 166篇 |
1998年 | 86篇 |
1997年 | 98篇 |
1996年 | 117篇 |
1995年 | 87篇 |
1994年 | 102篇 |
1993年 | 96篇 |
1992年 | 134篇 |
1991年 | 143篇 |
1990年 | 145篇 |
1989年 | 163篇 |
1988年 | 120篇 |
1987年 | 117篇 |
1986年 | 159篇 |
1985年 | 154篇 |
1984年 | 131篇 |
1983年 | 129篇 |
1982年 | 115篇 |
1981年 | 97篇 |
1980年 | 106篇 |
1979年 | 139篇 |
1978年 | 120篇 |
1977年 | 106篇 |
1976年 | 102篇 |
1974年 | 110篇 |
1973年 | 93篇 |
1972年 | 85篇 |
1971年 | 108篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
112.
113.
114.
115.
H. Stead 《Research Policy》1976,5(1):2-9
Since its publication in 1967, the estimates of the US Panel on Invention and Innovation (“Charpie” Panel) on the cost structure of technological innovation have been widely quoted and almost universally accepted. However, two later studies, by Edwin Mansfield and by Statistics Canada, have found that R&D accounts for a far greater proportion of total innovation costs than that suggested by the Charpie Panel. The Canadian study may be particularly interesting for medium-sized industrial nations since it is likely that the costs of commercializing the results of R&D are affected by the size of the potential market or field of utilization of the innovation. The results of Mansfield and Statistics Canada imply that governments, in their encouragement of innovation by supporting R&D, have been following a better strategy than perhaps they knew. 相似文献
116.
Recently, a model of communication theory and research has appeared in the literature within which stable individual differences in communication behavior represent individual differences in activation thresholds of neurobiological systems. The neurobiological systems thought to underly communication traits and behavior are assumed to be primarily due to genetic inheritance. As such, the model assigns a limited role to adaptability in social situations, instead positing communication adaptability as an inherited trait. In the present study, heritability estimates for the dimensions of communicative adaptability were derived from correlations based on identical and fraternal twins’ responses to a multidimensional communicative adaptability measure. Results indicated that social composure was 88% heritable, wit was 90% heritable, social confirmation was 37% heritable, articulation ability, and appropriate disclosure were 0% heritable. Theoretical implications are discussed. 相似文献
117.
Roundtable: what is temperament? Four approaches 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
H H Goldsmith A H Buss R Plomin M K Rothbart A Thomas S Chess R A Hinde R B McCall 《Child development》1987,58(2):505-529
4 current approaches to understanding temperament are discussed in the roundtable. In an introductory overview, Goldsmith outlines some of the major convergences and divergences in the understanding of this concept. Theorists representing 4 positions--Goldsmith, Buss and Plomin, Rothbart, and Thomas and Chess--outline their views by responding to each of 6 questions: How do you define temperament and explain the boundaries of the concept? What are the elements of temperatment? How does the construct of temperament permit you to approach issues or organize data in ways that are possible only if this construct is invoked? How does temperament develop? To what extent do you consider temperament to be a personological versus a relational or an interactional construct? and How does your approach deal with issues of temperamental "difficulty"? In 2 commentaries on the theorists' answers, Hinde highlights differences among their positions and indicates issues that current theories of temperament must take into consideration, and McCall draws on common aspects to propose a synthesizing definition that draws on all 4 approaches. 相似文献
118.
This article is the third in a series reporting on the changes in attitudes and interests‐‐mainly with respect to physics‐of several hundred students as they proceed through the Danish upper secondary school (Gymnasium). It concentrates on the results of the final investigation which took place shortly before the students leave the school. 相似文献
119.
Leslie D. Leve Allen A. Winebarger Beverly I. Fagot John B. Reid H. Hill Goldsmith 《Child development》1998,69(5):1286-1298
The genetic and environmental contributions to children's maladaptive behavior are assessed in a sample of 154 twin pairs (77 MZ twin pairs and 77 DZ twin pairs), who range in age from 6 to 11 years. To bridge the strengths of behavioral genetic methods and environmental assessment techniques, we use a multimethod, multimeasure approach to data collection, and analyze the data using behavioral genetic modeling techniques. Results indicate that genetic variation accounts for a majority of the variance in parent-reported child maladaptive behavior (average = 62%). One parent-report measure also suggests a smaller, significant contribution of shared environmental variance. In contrast to the parental ratings, the observational coding and global impressions of parent-twin interactive behavior suggest that shared environment is the primary source of variance accounting for parent and child maladaptive behavior. This is due, in part, to the direct influence one's interactive partner has on the expression of maladaptive behavior in an interactive setting. When controlling for the co-participant's behavior, genetic variation increases and shared environmental variation decreases. 相似文献
120.
Mary T. Rourke Robert H. Wozniak Kimberly Wright Cassidy 《Early education and development》1999,10(2):209-227
This study sought to demonstrate that many aspects of preschoolers' peer conflict behavior are sensitive to the subtle change in social situation that occurs when a child plays with a different partner. Twenty-seven pairs of preschoolers were videotaped over four 15-minute play sessions; in the fourth session, half of the pairs played with a different partner, while half were with the same partner. Frequency and theme of conflicts, strategies used, frequency of yielding to partner, and percentage of conflicts solved were observed. Initiating and negotiating behavior in early sessions better predicted behavior in the fourth session for the same-partner (vs. change-partner) group, and thus emerged as behaviors sensitive to social context changes. Results indicate that preschoolers have a remarkably varied array of conflict behaviors and that much of their conflict behavior is sensitive to influences inherent to social interaction with a specific partner. Ways in which the apparent sophistication in young children's social understanding can inform developmental approaches to early childhood education as well as both preventative and therapeutic conflict resolution programs are discussed. 相似文献