首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2966篇
  免费   55篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   2147篇
科学研究   163篇
各国文化   44篇
体育   428篇
综合类   1篇
文化理论   70篇
信息传播   169篇
  2023年   28篇
  2022年   42篇
  2021年   73篇
  2020年   117篇
  2019年   192篇
  2018年   264篇
  2017年   240篇
  2016年   207篇
  2015年   136篇
  2014年   148篇
  2013年   669篇
  2012年   127篇
  2011年   87篇
  2010年   65篇
  2009年   57篇
  2008年   57篇
  2007年   53篇
  2006年   57篇
  2005年   32篇
  2004年   28篇
  2003年   28篇
  2002年   28篇
  2001年   24篇
  2000年   24篇
  1999年   15篇
  1998年   16篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   17篇
  1995年   16篇
  1994年   15篇
  1993年   10篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   13篇
  1989年   10篇
  1988年   4篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   3篇
  1974年   5篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   6篇
  1969年   3篇
  1963年   3篇
  1956年   3篇
  1872年   4篇
  1870年   4篇
排序方式: 共有3022条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
92.
93.
94.
The aims of this study were twofold: (1) to determine whether open feedback (i.e. the ability to hear or see the scores of colleague judges after each performance) would lead judges in gymnastics to conform with their colleagues, and (2) to identify the underlying process on which this conformity is based. Twenty-three certified Flemish judges in women's gymnastics were randomly divided into panels of a maximum of five judges. These panels had to rate the same 30 videotaped individual vaults: 15 in phase 1 and 15 in phase 2. Two independent variables were orthogonally manipulated: feedback (or no feedback) during phase 1 and feedback (or no feedback) during phase 2. The results of phase 1 revealed that the variation between the judges' scores was less within panels that had received feedback than within panels that had not received feedback. We therefore conclude that the availability of feedback elicits conformity among gymnastic judges. The results of phase 2 indicated that this conformity continued even when feedback was no longer provided, suggesting that the observed conformity was based on informational influencing (i.e. because of uncertainty about the correct responses) and not on normative influencing (i.e. out of fear of standing out in the group).  相似文献   
95.
The aims of this study were to investigate the energy build-up and dissipation mechanisms associated with using an arm swing in submaximal and maximal vertical jumping and to establish the energy benefit of this arm swing. Twenty adult males were asked to perform a series of submaximal and maximal vertical jumps while using an arm swing. Force, motion and electromyographic data were recorded during each performance and used to compute a range of kinematic and kinetic variables, including ankle, knee, hip, shoulder and elbow joint powers and work done. It was found that the energy benefit of using an arm swing appears to be closely related to the maximum kinetic energy of the arms during their downswing, and increases as jump height increases. As jump height increases, energy in the arms is built up by a greater range of motion at the shoulder and greater effort of the shoulder and elbow muscles but, as jump height approaches maximum, these sources are supplemented by energy supplied by the trunk due to its earlier extension in the movement. The kinetic energy developed by the arms is used to increase their potential energy at take-off but also to store and return energy from the lower limbs and to "pull" on the rest of the body. These latter two mechanisms become more important as jump height increases with the pull being the more important of the two. We conclude that an arm swing contributes to jump performance in submaximal as well as maximal jumping but the energy generation and dissipation sources change as performance approaches maximum.  相似文献   
96.
97.
98.
99.
100.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号