首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   23625篇
  免费   213篇
  国内免费   32篇
教育   17036篇
科学研究   2063篇
各国文化   242篇
体育   1609篇
综合类   46篇
文化理论   124篇
信息传播   2750篇
  2021年   250篇
  2020年   332篇
  2019年   517篇
  2018年   669篇
  2017年   675篇
  2016年   643篇
  2015年   418篇
  2014年   566篇
  2013年   4534篇
  2012年   568篇
  2011年   589篇
  2010年   521篇
  2009年   481篇
  2008年   523篇
  2007年   487篇
  2006年   508篇
  2005年   448篇
  2004年   355篇
  2003年   320篇
  2002年   307篇
  2001年   472篇
  2000年   381篇
  1999年   339篇
  1998年   216篇
  1997年   212篇
  1996年   241篇
  1995年   195篇
  1994年   199篇
  1993年   220篇
  1992年   307篇
  1991年   307篇
  1990年   332篇
  1989年   300篇
  1988年   253篇
  1987年   270篇
  1986年   273篇
  1985年   270篇
  1984年   259篇
  1983年   257篇
  1982年   226篇
  1981年   209篇
  1980年   208篇
  1979年   277篇
  1978年   240篇
  1977年   191篇
  1976年   186篇
  1975年   160篇
  1974年   160篇
  1973年   160篇
  1971年   166篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
It was suggested that fluency, defined as number of responses, may misleadingly influence both high Intercorrelations sometimes reported among measures of creativity and low correlations sometimes reported between measures of creativity and intelligence. Subjects were 93 Saturday art school students between the ages of 9 and 15 years. Intercorrelations among five “creativity” scores derived from a slightly modified version of Torrance's Figure Completion Test and between these scores and Henmon-Nelson Intelligence were compared using both raw creativity scores and creativity scores corrected for the effect of fluency. Uncorrected creativity scores intercorrelated high among themselves (mean r= .45) and low with intelligence (mean r= .09), while corrected creativity scores showed low intercorrelations among themselves (mean r= .08) and with intelligence (mean r= .13). These findings are interpreted as confirming the influence of fluency upon high intercorrelations among so-called measures of creativity and as failing to support the suggestion that fluency may also influence low correlations among creativity and intelligence measures.  相似文献   
72.
    
  相似文献   
73.
    
The results of a national survey regarding the preparation of secondary school counselors is presented. Included is information on core curriculum, elective courses, and practicum requirements.  相似文献   
74.
75.
This article reports on the development and evaluation of an instructional module for teaching systematic desensitization. As the module makes use of widely available published materials, it requires little classroom time or work for the instructor. Furthermore, the module gives the student an evaluated quasiclinical experience with desensitization. Physiological and self-report data reflecting changes in client fear levels were gathered in an attempt to determine whether the instructional module is effective.  相似文献   
76.
77.
    
This study examines the use of cross-classified random effects models (CCrem) and cross-classified multiple membership random effects models (CCMMrem) to model rater bias and estimate teacher effectiveness. Effect estimates are compared using CTT versus item response theory (IRT) scaling methods and three models (i.e., conventional multilevel model, CCrem, CCMMrem). Results indicate that ignoring rater bias can lead to teachers being misclassified within an evaluation system. The best estimates of teacher effectiveness are produced using CCrems regardless of scaling method. Use of CCMMrems to model rater bias cannot be recommended based on the results of this study; combining the use of CCMMrems with an IRT scaling method produced especially unstable results.  相似文献   
78.
Since Prestel and other televised print sources signal a revolution in the presentation of reading material to the public, it is necessary to ensure that the viewers are able to read the material. Are they ready and able to adapt their reading skills to read televised texts? Differences between page print and television print are discussed, with respect to form and style, and recommendations are made about the responsibility of the educational establishment towards the preparation of potential viewers.  相似文献   
79.
Goldfish trained to discriminate between signals paired with shock (S?) and signals paired with shock omission (S+) with a linear presentation procedure, originally learned (OL) to control the signal state of a shuttle box and showed a decided preference for the S+ signal. In Experiment 1, following OL, groups had one OL signal replaced (S+ or S?), both signals replaced (S+ and S?), or the OL signals reversed (S+ and S? reversed) and were then tested in a transfer training procedure. In transfer, groups with one signal replaced maintained discriminated performance at OL levels; the S+ replaced group was slightly superior to the S? replaced group on the first day of transfer. With both OL signals replaced, discrimination dropped to chance performance levels, whereas, with OL signal shock pairing reversed, discrimination performance dropped below chance levels. In Experiment 2, following OL, extinction procedures consisted of turning off the shocker (0% shock) or of shocking 100% or a random 25% of the trials. A fourth extinction procedure (R,) retained the trial start response-dependent shock-omission contingency, but shock differentiating the S+ and S? signals was eliminated entirely. Extinction of the S+/S? discrimination was measured both during extinction training per se and with reversal retraining of the S+/S? discrimination later. Groups for which the OL S+ was paired with shock during extinction extinguished on both measures, but groups for which the OL S? was paired with shock omission did not extinguish, especially as shown by the reversal test procedure. Theoretical implications and the implications for several conditioning procedures are discussed.  相似文献   
80.
    
  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号