全文获取类型
收费全文 | 468篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
教育 | 376篇 |
科学研究 | 11篇 |
各国文化 | 9篇 |
体育 | 29篇 |
综合类 | 2篇 |
文化理论 | 2篇 |
信息传播 | 40篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 129篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 15篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有469条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
The primary purpose of this study was to examine the relation between full - time or part - time instructor status and college student retention and academic performance in sequential courses . Results indicate that for either developmental or regular courses, college students who take the first course in a sequence from a part - time instructor , and who take the second course in the sequence from a full - time instructor seem underprepared for the second course. By contrast to students experiencing other instructor status combinations (part - time / part - time , full - time / part - time , or full - time / full - time), these students are significantly less likely to either complete or achieve a grade of "C" or better in the second course. Sequential course instructor status, therefore, seems to be a predictor of college student success. Implications for practice pertaining to further research, college students, and institutions are discussed. 相似文献
54.
Shu-Mey Yu Larry D. Yore 《International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education》2013,11(5):1233-1254
The purpose of this study was to investigate the quality, evolution, and position of university students’ argumentation about organic agriculture over a 4-week argument–critique–argument e-learning experience embedded in a first year university biology course. The participants (N??=??43) were classified into three groups based on their epistemological views. Data collected from individual arguments, group deliberations, and individual critiques were coded and analyzed to establish the quality and evolution of argumentation. Results indicated significant improvement in the quality of their justifications between the first and second arguments. Post-hoc comparison of epistemological groups indicated that the more constructivist-oriented students had a greater significant evolution of their justifications than the more empiricist-oriented students, but there was no significant main effect for epistemological orientation. Qualitative analysis of the intervening critiques indicated that some students incorporated or used other students’ arguments or counter-arguments to change their position or to enhance the justification of their original position on organic agriculture, while others appeared to be locked into a confirmation-bias stance and search for evidence that supported their original position and disregarded contradictory evidence. 相似文献
55.
Vipin K. Agrawal Poonam Khanna Vijay K. Agrawal Larry W. Hughes 《Decision Sciences Journal of Innovative Education》2020,18(3):481-516
Ongoing curriculum change in higher education is essential to enhance student learning and better prepare them for the job‐market. However, research shows that faculty are reluctant to implement such changes because students generally react adversely thereby negating any potential benefits, and moreover, sanction faculty through lower evaluations and future enrollment. Yet, understanding of the effect of curriculum changes on students’ course and instructor perceptions is limited. In this article we attempt to fill this gap. Drawing on two empirical findings—students’ motivation to attend college becoming increasingly extrinsic since the 1960s and their inability to recognize, ex ante, the value‐added by a curriculum change—and the norm life‐cycle theory, we argue that any effort‐increasing or grade‐threatening change is viewed as non‐normative and will lead to an adverse student reaction. However, this adverse reaction will dissipate over time once a critical mass of students is convinced of the merits of the new curriculum. We find support for our hypotheses by analyzing change in student perceptions following curriculum changes at a U.S. University. In addition, we also find that once the adverse reaction dissipates, students’ perceptions of the new curriculum become more positive than the old curriculum, only to be reversed once the revised curriculum is accepted as the new norm. 相似文献
56.
57.
This article presents the results and an analysis of 1,728 survey responses from faculty at four top tier religious research universities regarding their support for moral and civic education. The results demonstrate that a majority of faculty members at these schools supported both the general goals of moral and civic education and specific suggestions for integrating moral and civic education into the curriculum. Nonetheless, three major concerns emerged about its actual implementation into the curriculum. 相似文献
58.
59.
WISC-Rs and Stanford-Binets were administered to 50 children with developmental disabilities referred for comprehensive evaluations. Although the two IQs correlated highly and significantly, it was found that 54% of the children received different classifications using the two instruments. Thus, testers should be aware that different classifications of intellectual level may be derived for the same child depending upon which test is used. 相似文献
60.
Larry Nucci 《Journal of moral education》2016,45(3):291-307
This article reasserts the centrality of reasoning as the focus for moral education. Attention to moral cognition must be extended to incorporate sociogenetic processes in moral growth. Moral education is not simply growth within the moral domain, but addresses capacities of students to engage in cross-domain coordination. Development beyond adolescence in moral thinking is in two forms: (1) the gradual application of morality in broader adult contexts, and (2) the result of social discourse and progressive readjustments at the individual and societal level of views of the morality of societal practices. Postconventional moral reasoning is not a rarified stage of moral cognition, but an orientation and set of discourse skills potentially available to all normally developing adult moral reasoners. 相似文献