首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   45869篇
  免费   788篇
  国内免费   776篇
教育   27263篇
科学研究   8418篇
各国文化   153篇
体育   2691篇
综合类   1991篇
文化理论   461篇
信息传播   6456篇
  2024年   7篇
  2023年   44篇
  2022年   423篇
  2021年   1041篇
  2020年   795篇
  2019年   368篇
  2018年   2516篇
  2017年   2547篇
  2016年   1967篇
  2015年   1322篇
  2014年   1991篇
  2013年   2470篇
  2012年   2943篇
  2011年   3578篇
  2010年   3450篇
  2009年   2732篇
  2008年   2965篇
  2007年   3708篇
  2006年   2584篇
  2005年   2449篇
  2004年   1502篇
  2003年   1310篇
  2002年   1061篇
  2001年   851篇
  2000年   546篇
  1999年   587篇
  1998年   377篇
  1997年   323篇
  1996年   237篇
  1995年   185篇
  1994年   150篇
  1993年   96篇
  1992年   80篇
  1991年   54篇
  1990年   40篇
  1989年   34篇
  1988年   19篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   9篇
  1984年   15篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   2篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   6篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   3篇
  1972年   1篇
  1957年   16篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
952.
This study examined changes in student motivation and achievement in science during a visit to a university children’s science museum. The study was based on the pretest–posttest control comparison group design with four treatment groups: control, exhibit, lesson and exhibit/lesson. The sample consisted of 228 sixth-grade students from a Louisiana public school who were randomly assigned to one of the four experimental groups. Pretest, posttest and delayed posttest measures of intrinsic motivation and achievement in science were obtained using the Children’s Academic Intrinsic Motivation Inventory and an achievement test written to measure areas of science incorporated in the museum exhibits. The data were analysed using a one-way ANOVA, dependent t tests and Pearson r. Significant differences were found within groups for (1) the lesson group in motivation and (2) the exhibit group in achievement from pretest to posttest and from posttest to delayed posttest. A significant relationship between level of motivation and science achievement was revealed for the exhibit group on the delayed posttest. There were no other significant findings to support that the treatment led to any long-term effects on motivation or achievement within any of the four experimental groups.  相似文献   
953.
954.
Identification of distinct units within a continuous flow of human action is fundamental to action processing. Such segmentation may rest in part on statistical learning. In a series of four experiments, we examined what types of statistics people can use to segment a continuous stream involving many brief, goal-directed action elements. The results of Experiment 1 showed no evidence for sensitivity to conditional probability, whereas Experiment 2 displayed learning based on joint probability. In Experiment 3, we demonstrated that additional exposure to the input failed to engender sensitivity to conditional probability. However, the results of Experiment 4 showed that a subset of adults—namely, those more successful at identifying actions that had been seen more frequently than comparison sequences—were also successful at learning conditional-probability statistics. These experiments help to clarify the mechanisms subserving processing of intentional action, and they highlight important differences from, as well as similarities to, prior studies of statistical learning in other domains, including language.  相似文献   
955.
In a Pavlovian conditioning situation, an initially neutral stimulus may be made excitatory by nonreinforced presentations in compound with an established conditioned excitor [i.e., second-order conditioning (SOC)]. The established excitor may be either a punctate cue or the training context. In four conditioned suppression experiments using rats, we investigated whether SOC phenomena parallel other cue interaction effects. In Experiment 1, we found that the response potential of a target stimulus was directly related to the intertrial interval when SOC was mediated by a punctate cue, and inversely related to the intertrial interval when SOC was mediated by the training context. Experiment 2 demonstrated that punctate- and context-mediated SOC are oppositely affected by posttraining context extinction, and Experiments 3 and 4 demonstrated that context- and punctate-mediated SOC are differentially affected by conditioned stimulus (Experiment 3) and unconditioned stimulus (Experiment 4) preexposure treatments. These findings parallel phenomena in conditioned inhibition and cue competition situations.  相似文献   
956.
分析了当前国内高职院校应用化工技术专业(民用爆破器材方向)教材存在的缺陷和不足,结合教育改革实践,提出了与企业合作并按照职业岗位能力开发成套教材的解决方法,同时进行了解决方法的可行性分析。  相似文献   
957.
宿舍党建工作是高职院校学生党建工作的核心因素,目前存在队伍力量薄弱,组织管理不够健全,基础设施不够完备等问题。为此,我们应该构建三层四级式宿舍党组织管理新平台,以宿舍党支部为依托,多渠道开展思政工作,进一步突出党员的先锋模范作用,鼓励学生社团组织进宿舍,创建富有特色的宿舍文化。  相似文献   
958.
Attempts at school reform and improvement have experienced difficulty creating and implementing feedback systems that energize and sustain change efforts. If the call for reform at all levels of education is to be met, attention must be given to establishing effective feedback mechanisms in educational institutions as they embark on improvement efforts. The purpose of this article is to describe application of an educational design metaphor to create a Knowledge Management System that provides teachers with timely and constructive feedback about their professional practice. This system includes the way in which a corpus of professional knowledge about differentiated content and instruction, classroom grouping, evidence-based practice and classroom management can be embedded in a set of tools that provides multiple stakeholders (teachers, students, administrators) the opportunity to generate feedback about instructional practice. Use of the approach is discussed within the broader context of Knowledge Management as a vehicle to extend the role of feedback in school reform and improvement within a long term pilot application in one school.  相似文献   
959.
960.
This paper presents the concept of critical flexibility as an alternative to eclecticism in instructional design. Eclecticism is often viewed as a persuasive alternative to theoretical orthodoxy (i.e., rigid use of a single perspective or process) due to the openness and flexibility it purports to offer. In contrast, the authors argue that eclecticism ignores or discourages critical reflection regarding background understanding (e.g., implicit assumptions and values) and perpetuates the lack of openness and flexibility commonly associated with orthodoxy. Critical flexibility, as an alternative to eclecticism, emphasizes an awareness of background understanding, but construes it as capable of being explicated, critically examined, adjusted in specific contexts, and refined or developed over time to facilitate increasingly flexible and effective design practices. The authors clarify the nature of critical flexibility as a general way of being involved in the design process, suggest how it helps overcome the traditional theory–practice split, and discuss several of its implications for scholarship and training.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号