首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   342篇
  免费   4篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   284篇
科学研究   6篇
各国文化   1篇
体育   19篇
文化理论   2篇
信息传播   35篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   11篇
  2018年   14篇
  2017年   5篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   7篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   78篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   14篇
  2010年   11篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   9篇
  2007年   8篇
  2006年   6篇
  2005年   8篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   11篇
  2002年   9篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   8篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   3篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   3篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   5篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   1篇
  1969年   1篇
  1965年   1篇
排序方式: 共有347条查询结果,搜索用时 11 毫秒
271.
Book review     
  相似文献   
272.
Abstract

The present study was designed to examine the effects of age, modeling and verbal self-instruction on children's performance of a sequential motor task. Eighty-four children between the ages of 4–0 and 5–11 years and 84 children between the ages of 7–0 and 8–11 years were randomly assigned to one of six instructional conditions in a 2 × 3 × 2 (age × model type × verbal self-instruction) factorial design. Results revealed that older children performed better than younger children on motor, verbal-cognitive, and attentional measures. More importantly, an age by model type interaction revealed that model effectiveness depended on the age of the observer as well as the type of model observed. Specifically, 7- and 8-year-old children performed equally well after observing either a silent or verbal model, while the 4- and 5-year-olds performed best under a verbal model only. The absence of verbal self-instructional effects was attributed to task difficulty, information processing capabilities and methodological procedures. In general, the results supported the notion that developmental factors play a critical role in the modeling process, and both theoretical and practical implications are outlined.  相似文献   
273.
The role of children's aggression and three indices of social competence (peer‐preferred behavior, teacher‐preferred behavior, and school adjustment) in children's perceived relationships with their teachers was assessed. Participants were 1,432 third through fifth graders (688 males, 744 females) and their teachers. The results from hierarchical regression analyses showed statistically significant interaction effects. Poor school adjustment was associated with more negatively perceived child teacher relationships for boys than for girls. In addition, the perceived child–teacher relationship among aggressive children was more favorable among those with high levels of school adjustment than among those who were poorly adjusted at school. Implications for school psychologists and teachers are discussed. © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.  相似文献   
274.
Two studies are reported concerning the height of male and female figures in children's drawings. In Study 1, 5 and 7 year olds were asked to draw a man and a lady or a boy and a girl. Adult figures were drawn taller than child figures. For boys, the mean heights of their male and female figures were approximately the same but girls drew their females taller than their male figures. The reason for this was that more girls than boys used different structures (incorporating a skirt or trousers into the drawing) to differentiate their figures; this use of different structures results in a taller drawing of a skirted figure. This finding was replicated in Study 2 in which 7-year-olds were asked to draw a lady in a skirt and another in pyjamas: the skirted figure was taller when it had a different structure from the figure in pyjamas, whereas the mean heights of both figures were approximately the same height when the same structure had been used. Deux travaux de recherche ont étudié la taille des silhouettes masculines et féminines dans les dessins d'enfants. Dans l'étude 1, des enfants de 5 et 7 ans ont dû dessiner un homme et une femme ou un garçon et une fille. Les silhouettes adultes ont été dessinées plus grandes que les silhouettes d'enfants. Les garçons ont dessiné des silhouettes d'hommes et de femmes à peu près de la même taille en moyenne, mais les filles ont dessiné leurs femmes plus grandes que leurs hommes. La raison en est que plus de filles que de garçons utilisaient des structures différentes (en incorporant une jupe ou un pantalon dans le dessin) pour différencier leurs silhouettes; cette utilisation de structures différentes résulte en un dessin plus haut de la silhouette en jupe. Ces résultats se retrouvent dans l'étude 2, où des enfants de 7 ans ont dû dessiner une dame en jupe et une autre en pyjama: la silhouette en jupe était plus haute quand elle avait une structure différente de celle en pyjama, tandis que la hauteur moyenne des deux silhouettes était à peu près la même quand la même structure était utilisée. Se informa de dos estudios relacionados con la altura de las figuras masculina y femenina en los dibujos de los niños. En el Estudio 1, se pidió a niños de 5 y 7 años que dibujan un hombre y una mujer, o un chico y una chica. Las figuras adultas fueron dibujadas con una altura mayor que las figuras de niños. Para los niños, las alturas medias de sus figuras masculinas y femeninas fueron aproximadamente las mismas, pero las niñas dibujaron sus figuras femeninas más altas que las masculinas. La razón para esto fue que más niñas que niños usaron estructuras diferentes (incorporando una falda o pantalones en el dibujo) para diferenciar sus figuras. El uso de estas estructuras diferentes resulta en un dibujo más alto de una figura con falda. Este hallazgo se reprodujo en el Estudio 2, en el que se pidió a niños de 7 años que dibujaran una señora con una falda y otra con un pijama: la figura con falda fue más alta cuando tuvo una estructura diferente a la de la figura con pijama, mientras que las alturas medias de ambas figuras fueron aproximadamente la misma altura cuando la hizo uso de la misma estructura.  相似文献   
275.
This article focuses on managing the client and the concultant with a performance appraisal process. The authors use their current practical experiences to consider the management of an effective client-consultant relationship. Dorothy writes from her perspective as a training director and user consultants and Maureen from her experience as an independent consultant providing management training and consulting services in the Metropolitan Washington area.  相似文献   
276.
The present research studied children in the second year of life (N = 29, Mage = 21.14 months, SD = 2.64 months) using experimental manipulations within and between subjects to show that responsive parental influence helps children have more frequent sustained object holds with fewer switches between objects compared to when parents are either not involved or over-involved. Regardless of parental involvement, sustained holds were visually rich, based on the size, centeredness, and dominance of the held object relative to other objects. These findings are important because they suggest not only that the child's body creates visually rich scenes across play contexts but also that a responsive parent can increase the frequency of these visually rich and informative moments.  相似文献   
277.
This article analyzes the conditions for mobilizing the science base for development of public policy. It does so by focusing upon the science-policy interface, specifically the processes of direct interaction between scientists and scientifically trained experts, on the one hand, and agents of policymaking organizations, on the other. The article defines two dimensions – cognitive distance and expert autonomy – which are argued to influence knowledge exchange, in such a way as to shape the outcome. A case study on the implementation of congestion charges in Stockholm, Sweden, illustrates how the proposed framework pinpoints three central issues for understanding these processes: (1) Differentiating the roles of, e.g., a science-based consultancy firm and an academic environment in policy formation; (2) Examining the fit between the organizational form of the science-policy interface and the intended goals; and (3) Increasing our understanding of when policymaker agents themselves need to develop scientific competence in order to interact effectively with scientific experts.  相似文献   
278.
279.
This qualitative research report adopts a critical pedagogy perspective to examine the provision of classroom accommodations for postsecondary students with learning disabilities. Although instructors in the United States are bound to abide by disability rights laws, we also believe instructors can act in ways that allow students to feel comfortable in disclosing their disabilities and in requesting and accessing accommodations for these disabilities. We engaged the voices of 10 university students living with learning disabilities through a series of semi‐structured interviews. These students offered a variety of statements on the ways that their disabilities were accommodated or not by their instructors. We classified these perceptions into three kinds of accommodation perceived by university students with learning disabilities: non‐accommodation, formal accommodation and accommodation for all students. We discuss the implications that these types of accommodations have for pedagogy and offer recommendations for effective techniques for accommodating for all. We hope the voices of these students will serve to enhance communication between students with learning disabilities and their professors.  相似文献   
280.
This study compares test-retest reliability and peak exercise responses from ramp-incremented (RAMP) and maximal perceptually-regulated (PRETmax) exercise tests during arm crank exercise in individuals reliant on manual wheelchair propulsion (MWP). Ten untrained participants completed four trials over 2-weeks (two RAMP (0–40 W + 5–10 W · min?1) trials and two PRETmax. PRETmax consisted of five, 2-min stages performed at Ratings of Perceived Exertion (RPE) 11, 13, 15, 17 and 20). Participants freely changed the power output to match the required RPE. Gas exchange variables, heart rate, power output, RPE and affect were determined throughout trials. The V?O2peak from RAMP (14.8 ± 5.5 ml · kg?1 · min?1) and PRETmax (13.9 ± 5.2 ml · kg?1 · min?1) trials were not different (P = 0.08). Measurement error was 1.7 and 2.2 ml · kg?1 · min?1 and coefficient of variation 5.9% and 8.1% for measuring V?O2peak from RAMP and PRETmax, respectively. Affect was more positive at RPE 13 (P = 0.02), 15 (P = 0.01) and 17 (P = 0.01) during PRETmax. Findings suggest that PRETmax can be used to measure V?O2peak in participants reliant on MWP and leads to a more positive affective response compared to RAMP.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号