首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   20009篇
  免费   265篇
  国内免费   26篇
教育   14240篇
科学研究   1910篇
各国文化   224篇
体育   1658篇
综合类   10篇
文化理论   208篇
信息传播   2050篇
  2021年   210篇
  2020年   349篇
  2019年   483篇
  2018年   608篇
  2017年   625篇
  2016年   612篇
  2015年   404篇
  2014年   501篇
  2013年   3737篇
  2012年   474篇
  2011年   517篇
  2010年   435篇
  2009年   383篇
  2008年   459篇
  2007年   414篇
  2006年   389篇
  2005年   376篇
  2004年   370篇
  2003年   344篇
  2002年   313篇
  2001年   557篇
  2000年   402篇
  1999年   324篇
  1998年   187篇
  1997年   249篇
  1996年   225篇
  1995年   183篇
  1994年   220篇
  1993年   176篇
  1992年   261篇
  1991年   259篇
  1990年   253篇
  1989年   256篇
  1988年   227篇
  1987年   203篇
  1986年   224篇
  1985年   267篇
  1984年   224篇
  1983年   208篇
  1982年   181篇
  1981年   160篇
  1980年   156篇
  1979年   213篇
  1978年   180篇
  1977年   148篇
  1976年   121篇
  1975年   130篇
  1974年   110篇
  1973年   117篇
  1971年   107篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
以广西82名业余体校男运动员为研究对象,运用数理统计方法,探讨了快肌百分比与有关身体素质指标和生理指标的相关程度,以及拟合的指标对快肌百分比的影响,旨在为速度性运动项目的选材提供理论依据。  相似文献   
12.
13.
The purpose of this study was to determine test-retest reliability for the 1-mile, 3/4-mile, and 1/2-mile distance run/alk tests for children in Grades K-4. Fifty-one intact physical education classes were randomly assigned to one of the three distance run conditions. A total of 1,229 (621 boys, 608 girls) completed the test-retests in the fall (October), with 1,050 of these students (543 boys, 507 girls) repeating the tests in the spring (May). Results indicated that the 1-mile run/walk distance, as recommended for young children in most national test batteries, has acceptable intraclass reliability (.83 less than R less than .90) for both boys and girls in Grades 3 and 4, has minimal (fall) to acceptable (spring) reliability for Grade 2 students (.70 less than R less than .83), but is not reliable for children in Grades K and 1 (.34 less than R less than .56). The 1/2 mile was the only distance meeting minimal reliability standards for boys and girls in Grades K and 1 (.73 less than R less than .82). Results also indicated that reliability estimates remained fairly stable across gender and age groups from the fall to spring testing periods, with the exception of the noticeably improved values for Grade 2 students on the 1-mile run/walk test. Criterion-referenced reliability (P, percent agreement) was also estimated relative to Physical Best and Fitnessgram run/walk standards. Reliability coefficients for all age group standards were acceptable to high (.70 less than P less than .95), except for Fitnessgram standards for 5-year-old girls on the 1-mile test for both fall and spring and for 6-year-old boys and girls on the 1-mile test administered in the spring.  相似文献   
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
The aim was to examine cross-cultural variation in linguistic responsiveness to young children in 10 English-speaking mother–child dyads and 10 Italian-speaking mother– child dyads. All 20 children were late talkers who possessed delays in expressive vocabulary development but age-appropriate cognitive and receptive language skills. Dyads were filmed in 15 minute free play contexts, which were transcribed and coded for measures of maternal linguistic input (e.g. rate, MLU, labels, expansions) and child language productivity (e.g. utterances, different words used). The results revealed that the Italian mothers used more utterances, spoke more quickly and used a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian mothers. The Italian children mirrored their mothers and also used more utterances and a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian children. Mothers in both groups used similar percentages of responsive labels and expansions. However, Italian mothers responded to fewer of their children's vocalisations, using a smaller percentage of imitations and interpretations than the Canadian mothers. Correlations between maternal input and children's language productivity revealed that contingent language measures (e.g. imitations, interpretations, expansions) were related to high levels of productivity in children in both cultural groups. The results support the use of language interventions based on increasing maternal responsiveness for these children at the one-word stage of language development. They also point to differences that may be culturally based. For example, Italian mothers use faster rates of interaction and appear to have higher expectations for their children's verbal participation in interaction. This is reflected in higher rates of language production from their children, even though children in both cultural groups have similar vocabulary sizes.  相似文献   
19.
20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号