首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8646篇
  免费   138篇
  国内免费   7篇
教育   6384篇
科学研究   521篇
各国文化   132篇
体育   695篇
综合类   4篇
文化理论   138篇
信息传播   917篇
  2023年   21篇
  2022年   46篇
  2021年   92篇
  2020年   195篇
  2019年   278篇
  2018年   297篇
  2017年   348篇
  2016年   325篇
  2015年   203篇
  2014年   267篇
  2013年   1894篇
  2012年   236篇
  2011年   250篇
  2010年   204篇
  2009年   205篇
  2008年   249篇
  2007年   236篇
  2006年   196篇
  2005年   208篇
  2004年   172篇
  2003年   184篇
  2002年   172篇
  2001年   145篇
  2000年   165篇
  1999年   110篇
  1998年   92篇
  1997年   106篇
  1996年   109篇
  1995年   85篇
  1994年   94篇
  1993年   95篇
  1992年   112篇
  1991年   95篇
  1990年   91篇
  1989年   74篇
  1988年   81篇
  1987年   69篇
  1986年   74篇
  1985年   87篇
  1984年   76篇
  1983年   74篇
  1982年   66篇
  1981年   63篇
  1980年   56篇
  1979年   66篇
  1978年   68篇
  1977年   40篇
  1976年   42篇
  1975年   28篇
  1974年   20篇
排序方式: 共有8791条查询结果,搜索用时 390 毫秒
71.
ABSTRACT

The literature on momentum is still undecided, with mixed results whether momentum exists or is only perceived to exist (“hot hand fallacy”). We explore whether momentum exists by looking at cases in which a basketball player has three consecutive free throws. A free throw is a well-defined task executed in a stable environment, allegedly giving momentum optimal chances to occur. Taking 14 NBA seasons we collected over 4500 three-free-throw sets (triplets). We obtained the outcomes of the shots as well as some additional variables about the player and the game: the player’s average free-throw percentage, home or away, and the game score and the quarter when the free throws were attempted. We first analyse the hit rates in the three shots and then proceed to regression analysis that also controls for the abovementioned variables. We address several concerns raised in the literature. All comparisons and analyses yield the same conclusion that there is no evidence for momentum in the data.  相似文献   
72.
73.
74.
75.
76.
In collaborative problem solving, children produce and evaluate arguments for proposals. We investigated whether 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 192) can produce and evaluate arguments against those arguments (i.e., counter-arguments). In Study 1, each child within a peer dyad was privately given a reason to prefer one over another solution to a task. One child, however, was given further information that would refute the reasoning of their partner. Five-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, identified and produced valid and relevant counter-arguments. In Study 2, 3-year-olds were given discourse training (discourse that contrasted valid and invalid counter-arguments) and then given the same problem-solving tasks. After training, 3-year-olds could also identify and produce valid and relevant counter-arguments. Thus, participating in discourse about reasons facilitates children’s counter-argumentation.  相似文献   
77.
78.
79.
At the University of Maine, middle and high school teachers observed more than 250 university science, technology, engineering, and mathematics classes and collected information on the nature of instruction, including how clickers were being used. Comparisons of classes taught with (n = 80) and without (n = 184) clickers show that, while instructional behaviors differ, the use of clickers alone does not significantly impact the time instructors spend lecturing. One possible explanation stems from the observation of three distinct modes of clicker use: peer discussion, in which students had the opportunity to talk with one another during clicker questions; individual thinking, in which no peer discussion was observed; and alternative collaboration, in which students had time for discussion, but it was not paired with clicker questions. Investigation of these modes revealed differences in the range of behaviors, the amount of time instructors lecture, and how challenging the clicker questions were to answer. Because instructors can vary their instructional style from one clicker question to the next, we also explored differences in how individual instructors incorporated peer discussion during clicker questions. These findings provide new insights into the range of clicker implementation at a campus-wide level and how such findings can be used to inform targeted professional development for faculty.  相似文献   
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号