首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   342篇
  免费   10篇
教育   262篇
科学研究   21篇
各国文化   2篇
体育   21篇
文化理论   8篇
信息传播   38篇
  2023年   4篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   12篇
  2017年   13篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   7篇
  2013年   77篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   10篇
  2010年   5篇
  2009年   9篇
  2008年   4篇
  2007年   4篇
  2006年   5篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   11篇
  2003年   9篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   6篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   3篇
  1988年   2篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   6篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   8篇
  1975年   3篇
  1973年   2篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   2篇
  1968年   3篇
  1898年   2篇
  1889年   2篇
排序方式: 共有352条查询结果,搜索用时 687 毫秒
91.
92.
93.
Establishing stability, defined as observing minimal measurement error in a test-retest assessment, is vital to validating psychometric tools. Correlational methods, such as Pearson product-moment, intraclass, and kappa are tests of association or consistency, whereas stability or reproducibility (regarded here as synonymous) assesses the agreement between test-retest scores. Indexes of reproducibility using the Task and Ego Orientation in Sport Questionnaire (TEOSQ; Duda & Nicholls, 1992) were investigated using correlational (Pearson product-moment, intraclass, and kappa) methods, repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance, and calculating the proportion of agreement within a referent value of +/-1 as suggested by Nevill, Lane, Kilgour, Bowes, and Whyte (2001). Two hundred thirteen soccer players completed the TEOSQ on two occasions, 1 week apart. Correlation analyses indicated a stronger test-retest correlation for the Ego subscale than the Task subscale. Multivariate analysis of variance indicated stability for ego items but with significant increases in four task items. The proportion of test-retest agreement scores indicated that all ego items reported relatively poor stability statistics with test-retest scores within a range of +/-1, ranging from 82.7-86.9%. By contrast, all task items showed test-retest difference scores ranging from 92.5-99%, although further analysis indicated that four task subscale items increased significantly. Findings illustrated that correlational methods (Pearson product-moment, intraclass, and kappa) are influenced by the range in scores, and calculating the proportion of agreement of test-retest differences with a referent value of +/-1 could provide additional insight into the stability of the questionnaire. It is suggested that the item-by-item proportion of agreement method proposed by Nevill et al. (2001) should be used to supplement existing methods and could be especially helpful in identifying rogue items in the initial stages of psychometric questionnaire validation.  相似文献   
94.
Book reviews     
Robert Fox  Terry Shinn 《Minerva》1981,19(2):329-339
  相似文献   
95.
96.
97.
Robert Fox 《Metascience》2014,23(2):365-367
  相似文献   
98.
99.
Memory for former preschool classmates was examined in 9-and 10-year-old children using measures of both explicit and implicit memory. Yes-no recognition of faces was at low levels but was significantly above chance. Skin conductance data showed similarly low but above-chance differentiation between classmates and controls. A median split of children based on their recognition memory scores showed that children with little evidence of explicit memory for classmates were as likely to show differential skin conductance responding to faces of former classmates as were children with higher recognition scores. Infantile amnesia, like the clinical syndrome for which it is named, may not always involve complete loss of encoded information.  相似文献   
100.
This study examined how timing (i.e., relative maturity) and rate (i.e., how quickly infants attain proficiency) of A‐not‐B performance were related to changes in brain activity from age 6 to 12 months. A‐not‐B performance and resting EEG (electroencephalography) were measured monthly from age 6 to 12 months in 28 infants and were modeled using logistic and linear growth curve models. Infants with faster performance rates reached performance milestones earlier. Infants with faster rates of increase in A‐not‐B performance had lower occipital power at 6 months and greater linear increases in occipital power. The results underscore the importance of considering nonlinear change processes for studying infants’ cognitive development as well as how these changes are related to trajectories of EEG power.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号