首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   517篇
  免费   8篇
教育   409篇
科学研究   18篇
各国文化   7篇
体育   45篇
文化理论   4篇
信息传播   42篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   16篇
  2018年   28篇
  2017年   18篇
  2016年   16篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   19篇
  2013年   109篇
  2012年   17篇
  2011年   17篇
  2010年   14篇
  2009年   24篇
  2008年   27篇
  2007年   17篇
  2006年   18篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   15篇
  2003年   15篇
  2002年   14篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   3篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1974年   2篇
  1971年   1篇
排序方式: 共有525条查询结果,搜索用时 8 毫秒
131.
The second to fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) is a sexually dimorphic trait with lower finger ratios considered more masculine. These digit ratios are believed to reflect the prenatal hormonal environment with higher exposure to androgens in utero leading to more masculine digit ratios. The 2D:4D ratio has been negatively correlated with many factors, including aggression, physical fitness, and athleticism. We compared 2D:4D finger ratios of (1) male and female varsity athletes (n = 99) versus male and female student non-athletes (n = 122), and (2) males (n = 104) versus females (n = 117). Our results confirmed that both male (mean ± s(x) : 0.97 ± 0.004) and female (0.98 ± 0.005) varsity athletes had significantly lower ratios than their non-varsity peers (males: 0.99 ± 0.004; females: 1.00 ± 0.006), and that male athletes had significantly lower 2D:4D ratios than female athletes. Overall, males had significantly lower 2D:4D ratios than females (0.98 ± 0.003 vs. 0.99 ± 0.004). A smaller 2D:4D ratio appears to be consistent with participation in varsity sports among both males and females.  相似文献   
132.
133.
134.
135.
136.
Over the last twenty-five years, there has been an increase in the availability of published checklists and schedules which allow practitioners to identify the strengths of children and young people, including those with special educational needs. While helpful, these assessments are unable to tell us about the nature of contextual factors which support the expression of particular strengths. In this study, we took a situative perspective to explore how specific classroom practices facilitated strengths. A multiple case study design was used to analyse practices nominated by three children/young people with special educational needs. Qualitative analysis revealed how aspects of practice afforded the opportunity for each child to participate in ways which they equated with their strengths. This style of strength-based assessment led to a more sustained examination of supportive practice features than would have been achieved through interview alone.  相似文献   
137.
138.
139.
The purpose of this study is to test for the presence of order-effect bias in journal ranking surveys. Data were obtained from 379 active knowledge management and intellectual capital researchers who rated 25 journals on a 7-point scale. Five different versions of the survey instrument were utilized. Consistent with the cognitive elaboration model, the satisficing theory, and the Gricean maxim of orderliness, order-effect bias was observed in journal ranking surveys. Journals that appear in the beginning of the ranking list delivered to survey respondents consistently receive higher scores than journals at the end of the list. Overall, the position of the journal in the list explains over 10% of its score. Therefore, authors of journal ranking studies are recommended to use multiple versions of the survey instrument with randomized journal orders.  相似文献   
140.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号