首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   23815篇
  免费   260篇
  国内免费   89篇
教育   17003篇
科学研究   2000篇
各国文化   220篇
体育   1881篇
综合类   222篇
文化理论   175篇
信息传播   2663篇
  2022年   231篇
  2021年   382篇
  2020年   464篇
  2019年   661篇
  2018年   840篇
  2017年   855篇
  2016年   791篇
  2015年   578篇
  2014年   754篇
  2013年   4273篇
  2012年   821篇
  2011年   794篇
  2010年   730篇
  2009年   652篇
  2008年   678篇
  2007年   731篇
  2006年   697篇
  2005年   618篇
  2004年   378篇
  2003年   405篇
  2002年   388篇
  2001年   515篇
  2000年   372篇
  1999年   340篇
  1998年   221篇
  1997年   190篇
  1996年   216篇
  1995年   183篇
  1994年   182篇
  1993年   156篇
  1992年   228篇
  1991年   224篇
  1990年   252篇
  1989年   234篇
  1988年   190篇
  1987年   196篇
  1986年   195篇
  1985年   181篇
  1984年   188篇
  1983年   176篇
  1982年   144篇
  1981年   146篇
  1980年   136篇
  1979年   200篇
  1978年   168篇
  1977年   118篇
  1976年   130篇
  1975年   118篇
  1973年   109篇
  1971年   120篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
Response-to-intervention (RTI) approaches to disability identification are meant to put an end to the so-called wait-to-fail requirement associated with IQ discrepancy. However, in an unfortunate irony, there is a group of children who wait to fail in RTI frameworks. That is, they must fail both general classroom instruction (Tier 1) and small-group intervention (Tier 2) before becoming eligible for the most intensive intervention (Tier 3). The purpose of this article was to determine how to predict accurately which at-risk children will be unresponsive to Tiers 1 and 2, thereby allowing unresponsive children to move directly from Tier 1 to Tier 3. As part of an efficacy study of a multitier RTI approach to prevention and identification of reading disabilities (RD), 129 first-grade children who were unresponsive to classroom reading instruction were randomly assigned to 14 weeks of small-group, Tier 2 intervention. Nonresponders to this instruction (n = 33) were identified using local norms on first-grade word identification fluency growth linked to a distal outcome of RD at the end of second grade. Logistic regression models were used to predict membership in responder and nonresponder groups. Predictors were entered as blocks of data from least to most difficult to obtain: universal screening data, Tier 1 response data, norm referenced tests, and Tier 2 response data. Tier 2 response data were not necessary to classify students as responders and nonresponders to Tier 2 instruction, suggesting that some children can be accurately identified as eligible for Tier 3 intervention using only Tier 1 data, thereby avoiding prolonged periods of failure to instruction.  相似文献   
182.
Resumen

El déficit atencional en niños hiperactivos ha sido investigado a partir de las teorías tradicionales de la atención. Este trabajo incorpora las recientes teorías de limitaciones en la capacidad de procesamiento mediante un paradigma de doble tarea (visual y auditiva). Se manipula el nivel de dificultad de la tarea secundaria y se comprueba su efecto en niños hiperactivos (n-=28) y normales (n=28) de 2.°-3.° y 4.°-6.° cursos de EGB. Los resultados permiten sostener que al aumentar la dificultad de la tarea secundaria, los hiperactivos de 4.°-6.° tienen peor ejecución que los controles de los mismos cursos y edad, mientras que en 2.°-3.° ocurre lo contrario. Los hiperactivos muestran una mayor inestabilidad y labilidad en la asignación de recursos y/o en los límites de capacidad de procesamiento.  相似文献   
183.
Measurements of formal reasoning and principled moral reasoning ability were obtained from a sample of 99 tenth grade students. Specific modes of formal reasoning (proportional reasoning, controlling variables, probabilistic, correlational, and combinatorial reasoning) were first examined. Findings support the notion of hierarchical relationships which exist among those variables. Further results from factor analysis provide evidence that the variables represent specific cognitive structures that are interdependent with each other and precede operations in development. Finally, significant relationships were found to exist between the different modes of formal reasoning and principled moral reasoning. Combinatorial and correlational reasoning were found to significantly account for 22% of the variance in principled moral reasoning. Theoretical and educational implications are discussed.  相似文献   
184.
    
Accountability mandates often prompt assessment of student learning gains (e.g., value-added estimates) via achievement tests. The validity of these estimates have been questioned when performance on tests is low stakes for students. To assess the effects of motivation on value-added estimates, we assigned students to one of three test consequence conditions: (a) an aggregate of test scores is used solely for institutional effectiveness purposes, (b) personal test score is reported to the student, or (c) personal test score is reported to faculty. Value-added estimates, operationalized as change in performance between two testing occasions for the same individuals where educational programming was experienced between testing occasions, were examined across conditions, in addition to the effects of test-taking motivation. Test consequences did not impact value-added estimates. Change in test-taking motivation, however, had a substantial effect on value-added estimates. In short, value-added estimates were attenuated due to decreased motivation from pretest to posttest.  相似文献   
185.
Se analiza a continuación la polémica generada en torno a la educación moral y cívica a lo largo de las últimas décadas y, en especial, la derivada de la LOGSE al dotar a la Etica de un carácter transversal generando nuevos planteamientos y perspectivas que procuran su mayor sistematización en el curriculum escolar.  相似文献   
186.
187.
    
  相似文献   
188.
The view that imagery might be a disadvantage on certain tasks might surprise some mathematics educators who contend that a learner's conceptual understanding is increased whenever visual imagery is used. One of the limitations of imagery found in the literature comes to bear on a unique aspect of mathematics teaching and learning. This is the notion of an uncontrollable image, which may persist, thereby preventing the opening up of more fruitful avenues of thought, a difficulty which is particularly acute if the image is vivid. Although one calculus student's images supported high levels of mathematical functioning, occasionally his vivid images became uncontrollable, and the power of these images did more to obscure than to explain. This type of imagery can be a major hindrance in constructing meaning for mathematical concepts — contrary to the panacea view of imagery which is sometimes expressed.This revised version was published online in September 2005 with corrections to the Cover Date.  相似文献   
189.
Children in public schools challenge people’s conceptions of them by talking about their spiritualities and spiritual practices. Based on a one-year multicultural feminist critical narrative inquiry, this article examines how Black and Latina/o first grade children co-researchers interview family members to think about their beliefs, encourage others, and to acquire more spiritual knowledge. I provide three counterstories in which children choose a mother, an older sister, and a younger brother to interview. These counterstories demonstrate children’s diverse literacies and spiritual practices, and families’ involvement in children’s lives. I discuss implications for educational practices in support of Black and Latina/o children and their families. I highlight ways pedagogical practices can be critiqued and transformed in order to better support children.Dr. Nadjwa E. L. Norton is an Assistant Professor in the Literacy Department at City College, CUNY. Her scholarship focuses on multiple literacies practices, spirituality, teacher education, equity-oriented multicultural education, and collaborative qualitative research designs. Address correspondence to Dr. Nadjwa E. L. Norton, Department of Literacy NAC 6/207, The City College of New York, 138th Street & Convent Avenue, New York, NY 10031, USA; e-mail: nnorton@ccny.cuny.edu.  相似文献   
190.
    
Previously University Director of Student Welfare and Students' Concerns at that university. Has also worked as Vocational Counsellor and teacher at several universities in Peru. Is interested in psychological research and in vocational and psychological guidance.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号