首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   370篇
  免费   3篇
教育   295篇
科学研究   13篇
各国文化   3篇
体育   16篇
文化理论   2篇
信息传播   44篇
  2023年   3篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   11篇
  2019年   18篇
  2018年   18篇
  2017年   16篇
  2016年   15篇
  2015年   13篇
  2014年   13篇
  2013年   78篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   10篇
  2010年   15篇
  2009年   13篇
  2008年   5篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   10篇
  2004年   8篇
  2003年   9篇
  2002年   16篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   5篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1992年   5篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1982年   3篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   4篇
  1961年   1篇
  1951年   2篇
  1949年   1篇
  1945年   1篇
  1943年   1篇
  1940年   1篇
  1939年   1篇
  1938年   1篇
  1934年   1篇
  1933年   1篇
  1926年   1篇
排序方式: 共有373条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
考研无疑是大学生们毕业时选择人数最多的道路之一,从这个实际情况来看,他最适合绝大多数同学.这是什么原因呢?我们在这里尝试综合一下,也许是画蛇添足,但也许能给低年级的同学们一点启发.  相似文献   
42.
The purpose of this study was to examine second graders’ (n = 680) changing spoken nonmainstream American English (NMAE) use in relation to their oral language and reading comprehension achievement. Fall NMAE production was negatively associated with fall achievement scores. NMAE production generally decreased from fall to spring. Students who qualified for the US Free and Reduced Lunch program (FARL) and who had stronger language skills were more likely to decrease their NMAE use (i.e., dialect shifting) than their peers who did not qualify for FARL or their peers with weaker language skills. Dialect shifting for a sub-sample of 102 students who used substantial amounts of NMAE at the beginning of the school year was predicted by school context, controlling for reading and language skills—in general, students who attended more affluent schools dialect shifted to a greater extent than did their peers who attended higher poverty schools. Greater dialect shifting in this group predicted gains in reading comprehension from fall to spring.  相似文献   
43.
This study examined the efficacy of using different types of mobile media apps to increase the receptive and expressive vocabulary development of 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old preschool children living in economically disadvantaged communities. Children and teachers in four Head Start classrooms participated in the quasi-experimental study, which included an 8-week intervention in which the children interacted with one of two types of apps: one classroom used four scaffolding-like vocabulary apps for an average of 1.8 h per day (n?=?16) and one classroom used four open-ended vocabulary apps for an average of 1.3 h per day (n?=?15). Two classrooms served as comparison groups (n?=?18; n?=?14) which used apps that were chosen by the Head Start program with no specific instructional method for approximately 1 h per day. Children’s vocabulary was assessed pre- and post-intervention using the PPVT-4, the EVT-2, and iPad Receptive and Expressive Vocabulary Assessments. Using a repeated measures analysis of variance with split plot analysis, children who used scaffolding-like vocabulary apps performed statistically significantly higher on the PPVT-4 than children who used open-ended vocabulary apps. There were no significant differences between groups on the EVT-2. The study contributed to the literature on teacher instruction of utilizing scaffolding-like apps to increase preschool vocabulary knowledge.  相似文献   
44.
The language children hear presents them with a multitude of co-occurrences between words and things in the world, and they must repeatedly determine which among these manifold co-occurrences is relevant. Social factors—such as cues regarding the speaker's referential intent—might serve as one guide to whether word-object covariation should be registered. In 2 studies, infants (15–20 months and 18–20 months in Studies 1 and 2, respectively) heard novel labels at a time when they were investigating a single novel object; in one case the label was uttered by a speaker seated within the infant's view and displaying concurrent attention to the novel toy (coupled condition), whereas in the other case the label emanated from a speaker seated out of the infant's view (decoupled condition). In both studies, subsequent comprehension questions indicated that infants of 18–20 months registered a stable link between label and object in the coupled condition, but not in the decoupled condition, despite the fact that covariation between label and object was equivalent in the 2 conditions. Thus, by 18–20 months children are inclined to establish a mapping between word and object only when a speaker displays signs of referring to that object.  相似文献   
45.
在选择考研的学校时,考哪个学校是和考哪个专业一样重要的问题,考名校还是考一般的学校,考本校还是外校,这是个问题.我的观点是,还是考名校比较好.  相似文献   
46.
Gender in plants     
Complex evolutionary forces determine whether plants are male, female or hermaphrodite throughout their lives or whether they switch from one sex to another. This article explores these often conflicting forces and seeks answers for sex change in plants.  相似文献   
47.
Gender in plants     
What is the difference between plant sex and plant gender? Why does stress sometimes cause maleness and other times result in femaleness? Do plant hedge mating bets using sex change? This article addresses these and other evolutionary questions about plant gender.  相似文献   
48.
49.
This article describes a secondary analysis of a brief reading comprehension rate measure, percent comprehension questions correct per minute spent reading (%C/M). This measure includes reading speed (seconds to read) in the denominator and percentage of comprehension questions answered correctly in the numerator. Participants were 22 4th‐, 29 5th‐, and 37 10th‐grade students. Results showed that reading speed accounted for much of the variance in Broad Reading Cluster scores and subtest scores of the Woodcock–Johnson III Tests of Achievement across all grade levels. Converting reading speed to the rate measure %C/M increased Broad Reading Cluster variance accounted for in the 4th‐ and 5th‐grade sample, but decreased the Broad Reading Cluster variance accounted for in the 10th‐grade sample. Discussion focuses on the importance of reading speed and the failure to enhance validity of a brief rate measure in more skilled readers by incorporating a direct measure of comprehension. © 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.  相似文献   
50.
As the number of young children displaying challenging behavior in early childhood grows, so too does the need to implement evidence-based practices that prevent challenging behavior. Positive Behavior Support (PBS) provides a framework of tiered interventions focused on promoting social-emotional development and preventing challenging behavior. This study investigated the effects of consultation on teachers’ implementation of universal PBS practices and children’s academic engagement. A multiple-baseline design was applied across four preschool classrooms serving children from 33 to 63 months of age. A strong relationship was documented between consultation and teachers’ implementation of PBS skills. High levels of academic engagement were maintained following consultation. Implications of the results are provided for applications of universal PBS practices in early childhood settings.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号