首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4302篇
  免费   47篇
  国内免费   6篇
教育   3337篇
科学研究   218篇
各国文化   82篇
体育   306篇
综合类   1篇
文化理论   61篇
信息传播   350篇
  2021年   24篇
  2020年   49篇
  2019年   81篇
  2018年   95篇
  2017年   123篇
  2016年   94篇
  2015年   83篇
  2014年   94篇
  2013年   1091篇
  2012年   71篇
  2011年   93篇
  2010年   70篇
  2009年   96篇
  2008年   108篇
  2007年   95篇
  2006年   99篇
  2005年   90篇
  2004年   82篇
  2003年   97篇
  2002年   80篇
  2001年   70篇
  2000年   71篇
  1999年   68篇
  1998年   59篇
  1997年   63篇
  1996年   65篇
  1995年   62篇
  1994年   51篇
  1993年   54篇
  1992年   50篇
  1991年   60篇
  1990年   52篇
  1989年   55篇
  1988年   43篇
  1987年   39篇
  1986年   46篇
  1985年   56篇
  1984年   31篇
  1983年   41篇
  1982年   41篇
  1981年   43篇
  1980年   32篇
  1979年   63篇
  1978年   31篇
  1977年   49篇
  1976年   33篇
  1975年   31篇
  1974年   37篇
  1973年   24篇
  1972年   22篇
排序方式: 共有4355条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
When judgmental and statistical procedures are both used to identify potentially gender-biased items in a test, to what extent do the results agree? In this study, both procedures were used to evaluate the items in a statewide, 78-item, multiple-choice test of science knowledge. Only one item was flagged by the sensitivity reviewers as being potentially biased, but this item was not flagged by the statistical procedure. None of the nine items flagged by the Mantel-Haenszel procedure were flagged by the sensitivity reviewers. Eight of the nine statistically flagged items were differentially easier for males. Four of these eight measured the same category of objectives. The authors conclude that both judgmental and statistical procedures provide useful information and that both should be used in test construction. They caution readers that content-validity issues need to be addressed when making decisions based on the results of either procedure.  相似文献   
82.
83.
84.
85.
86.
87.
The matching cloze procedure, which does not require language production skills and which is simple enough for the classroom teacher to construct unaided, was originally developed to measure reading skills of elementary English‐second‐language pupils. The results of this pilot study with opportunity school children indicate the validity of the procedure as an evaluation technique for slow learning children.  相似文献   
88.
Research in Science Education - Students learn more than science knowledge in a science classroom; they also acquire important messages about the purposes and contexts of science. Roberts (2011)...  相似文献   
89.
Pigeons pecked on three keys, responses to one of which could be reinforced after 3 flashes of the houselight, to a second key after 6, and to a third key after 12. The flashes were arranged according to variable-interval schedules. Response allocation among the keys was a function of the number of flashes. When flashes were omitted, transitions occurred very late. Increasing flash duration produced a leftward shift in the transitions along a number axis. Increasing reinforcement probability produced a leftward shift, and decreasing reinforcement probability produced a rightward shift. Intermixing different flash rates within sessions separated allocations: Faster flash rates shifted the functions sooner in real time, but later in terms of flash count, and conversely for slower flash rates. A model of control by fading memories of number and time was proposed.  相似文献   
90.
It is usually assumed that successful problemsolving in knowledge-rich domains depends onthe availability of abstract problem-typeschemas whose acquisition can be supported bypresenting students with worked examples.Conventionally designed worked examples oftenfocus on information that is related to themain components of problem-type schemas, namelyon information related to problem-categorymembership, structural task features, andcategory-specific solution procedures. However,studying these examples might be cognitivelydemanding because it requires learners tosimultaneously hold active a substantial amountof information in working memory. In ourresearch, we try to reduce intrinsic cognitiveload in example-based learning by shifting thelevel of presenting and explaining solutionprocedures from a `molar' view – that focuseson problem categories and their associatedoverall solution procedures – to a more`modular' view where complex solutions arebroken down into smaller meaningful solutionelements that can be conveyed separately. Wereview findings from five of our own studiesthat yield evidence for the fact thatprocessing modular examples is associated witha lower degree of intrinsic cognitive load andthus, improves learning.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号