首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1233篇
  免费   11篇
  国内免费   2篇
教育   993篇
科学研究   36篇
各国文化   20篇
体育   100篇
文化理论   25篇
信息传播   72篇
  2019年   20篇
  2018年   18篇
  2017年   37篇
  2016年   25篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   26篇
  2013年   267篇
  2012年   30篇
  2011年   31篇
  2010年   23篇
  2009年   39篇
  2008年   45篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   28篇
  2005年   24篇
  2004年   26篇
  2003年   21篇
  2002年   24篇
  2001年   20篇
  2000年   22篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   22篇
  1997年   20篇
  1996年   22篇
  1995年   12篇
  1994年   17篇
  1993年   15篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   15篇
  1988年   13篇
  1987年   14篇
  1986年   18篇
  1985年   16篇
  1984年   14篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   14篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   22篇
  1978年   16篇
  1977年   21篇
  1976年   16篇
  1975年   8篇
  1974年   10篇
  1973年   10篇
  1969年   6篇
  1967年   6篇
  1966年   6篇
排序方式: 共有1246条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
172.
173.
Recent advances in the neurosciences have begun to elucidate how some fundamental mechanisms of nervous system activity can explain human information processing and the acquisition of knowledge. Some of these findings are consistent with a cognitive view of constructivist models of learning and provide additional theoretical support for constructivist applications to science education reform. Current thought at the interface between neurocognitive research and constructivist philosophy is summarized here and discussed in a context of implications for scientific epistemology and conceptual change processes in science education.  相似文献   
174.
Modeling is a major topic of interest in mathematics education. However, the field’s definition of models is diverse. Less is known about what teachers identify as mathematical models, even though it is teachers who ultimately enact modeling activities in the classroom. In this study, we asked nine middle school teachers with a variety of academic backgrounds and teaching experience to collect data related to one familiar physical phenomenon, cooling liquid. We then asked each participant to construct a model of that phenomenon, describe why it was a model, and identify whether a variety of artifacts representing the phenomenon also counted as models during a semi-structured interview. We sought to identify: what do mathematics teachers attend to when describing what constitutes a model? And, how do their attentions shift as they engage in different activities related to models? Using content analysis, we documented what features and purposes teachers attended to when describing a mathematical model. When constructing their own model, they focused on the visual form of the model and what quantitative information it should include. When deciding whether particular representational artifacts constituted models, they focused on how the representations reflected the system under study, and what purposes those representations could serve in further understanding that system. These findings suggest teachers may have multiple understandings of models, which are active at different times and reflect different perspectives. This has implications for research, teacher education, and professional development.  相似文献   
175.
176.
In two experiments, visual and auditory memory was tested for good and for poor readers from the upper elementary grades. Under experimenter-blind conditions, no reading group differences existed for single mode presentation in recognition frequency or recognition latency. With a multimodal presentation, latencies for poor readers were similar to those for the single mode presentation. Good readers, however, had significantly faster latencies with multiple input. Generally, results supported dual coding and self-terminating memory search hypotheses for previously encoded stimuli. Implications of the latency differences between reader groups were discussed with respect to the reading process and reading theory.  相似文献   
177.
Consistent results of two experiments with relatively technical, native language vocabulary, showed that effectiveness of keyword methods depended upon whether meanings of words to be learned were abstract or concrete and whether comprehension was assessed immediately or after a delay. Keywords bearing acoustical similarities to vocabulary items whose meanings were presented as sentences consistently facilitated students’ retention of the meanings of concrete items on immediate posttests. However, in contrast to recent research on foreign language vocabulary having simpler definitions, these experiments showed that it may be difficult to use the keyword method to aid long-term retention with moderately complex. abstract, native language vocabulary. Parts of this study, which was partially supported by the Kansas State University Bureau of General Research, were presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Boston, 1980.  相似文献   
178.
179.
180.
OFSTED, Inspection and the Betrayal of Democracy   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Drawing briefly on the quite different discourses of schooling-as-performance and education-as-exploration, the paper opens by exploring some of the consequences of the distinction between schooling and education for any system of school inspection. The second section of the paper examines the conceptual and practical inadequacy of 'accountability' as an agent of reciprocal public engagement in a participatory democracy. In its stead a more robust, more open notion of 'reciprocal responsibility' is offered as a more fitting means of professional and communal renewal. Section III focuses on the relationship between means and ends that is at once central to democracy and so conspicuously absent from current inspection arrangements. The short conclusion suggests we need a radical break from OFSTED if we wish to approximate more closely to our democratic aspirations.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号