首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   987篇
  免费   2篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   819篇
科学研究   34篇
各国文化   8篇
体育   42篇
综合类   2篇
文化理论   14篇
信息传播   71篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   10篇
  2020年   19篇
  2019年   25篇
  2018年   32篇
  2017年   23篇
  2016年   31篇
  2015年   29篇
  2014年   28篇
  2013年   218篇
  2012年   26篇
  2011年   20篇
  2010年   18篇
  2009年   20篇
  2008年   21篇
  2007年   23篇
  2006年   16篇
  2005年   18篇
  2004年   13篇
  2003年   14篇
  2002年   18篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   17篇
  1999年   13篇
  1998年   14篇
  1997年   10篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   9篇
  1992年   20篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   17篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   12篇
  1986年   6篇
  1985年   10篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   8篇
  1980年   10篇
  1979年   12篇
  1978年   11篇
  1975年   6篇
  1973年   7篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   9篇
  1970年   11篇
  1968年   7篇
  1967年   5篇
排序方式: 共有990条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
901.
902.
The present study examined whether sublexical morphological processing takes place during visual word-recognition in Hebrew, and whether morphological decomposition of written words depends on lexical activation of the complete word. Furthermore, it examined whether morphological processing is similar when reading Hebrew as a first language (L1) or as a second language (L2), and whether L1’s morphological background, Semitic or Indo-European, modulates morphological processing in L2 Hebrew (a Semitic language), among proficient readers. To reveal the sublexical processing of the Hebrew morphemes, the Root (R) and the Pattern (P), a lexical-decision task was conducted, in which all critical stimuli were non-word letter-strings manipulated to include or exclude real Hebrew morphemes. Different combinations of real (+) and pseudo (?) morphemes yielded four types of non-words (+R+P; +R?P; ?R+P, ?R?P). Three groups of proficient Hebrew readers were tested: L1 Hebrew, L1 English-L2 Hebrew, and L1 Arabic-L2 Hebrew. Results demonstrated significant differences in latency and accuracy of responses to the four morphological conditions, indicating that sublexical morphological processing occurs during visual word-recognition of morphologically structured letter-strings in Hebrew. Importantly, the activation of real Hebrew morphemes occurred in non-word stimuli, indicating that morphological processing in Hebrew is separable from lexical activation. Moreover, the same pattern of results was observed in all three L1 groups, indicating that proficient L2 readers exhibit morphological processing strategies that are tuned to the L2 morphology, regardless of their L1 background.  相似文献   
903.
904.
Book reviews     
Rotter, Ekkehart, Abendland und Saratenen: das okxidentale Araberbild und seine Entetehung im Frühmittelalter. Berlin and New York: Walter de Gruyter, 1986. (Studien zur Sprache, Geschichte und Kultur des Islamischen Orients, neue Folge, Band 11) 290+xiipp. 2 genealogical charts; Sketch map; sketch; DM 158.

Irwin, Robert. The Middle East in the Middle Ages: The Early Mamlük Sultanate 1250–1382. Carbondale and Edwardsville: Southern Illinois University Press, 1986. 180 pp.: US $26.95.

Kobbervig, Karl I. El Libro de las Suertes: tratado de adivinación por el Juego de azar. Estudio, edición y glosario. Madrid: Gredos, 1987 (Colección de Literatura Española Aljamiado‐Morisca, 7), 227pp., including a full‐page facsimiles from the MSS.

Cutler, Allan Harris and Cutler, Helen Elmquist. The Jaw as Ally of the Muslim: Medieval Roots of Anti‐Semitism. Notre Dame, Indiana: University of Notre Dame Press, 1986, x + 577 pp., Indices; bibliography; US $50.00

Parker, S. Thomas. Romans and Saracens: A History of the Arabian Frontier. (American Schools of Oriental Research 1986. Dissertation Series, 6) Winona Lake, Indiana: Eisenbrauns,1987. xiii + 247pp.: US $25.00.

De Montêquin, François‐Auguste (compilar). Muslim Architecture of the Ibrian Peninsula: Eastern and Wastern Sources for Hispano‐Islamic Building Arts. West Cornwall. Locust Hill Press, 1987, xv + 241 pp.; US $30.00.  相似文献   

905.
Limited research has examined the skills of children with a reading disability (RD) and children with RD and a mathematics disability (MD). Even less research has examined the phonological awareness (PA) and rapid automatized naming (RAN) skills in these two groups of children and how these skills relate to reading and math achievement. Additionally, various classification criteria are frequently implemented to classify children with MD. The purpose of this study, therefore, was to examine the PA and RAN skills in children who met different criteria for RD only and children with RD who are at risk for mathematics difficulties (MDR). Participants were 114 second‐ or third‐grade students with RD from public elementary schools in three large metropolitan areas. Students were classified as at risk for mathematics difficulties utilizing a 25th‐percentile cutoff and a 15th‐percentile cutoff as assessed by the KeyMath‐Revised Test ( Connolly, 1988 ). A series of PA and RAN measures were administered along with a range of reading and mathematics measures. Hierarchical regression analyses indicated that children with RD only evidenced a different pattern of results compared to children with RD + MDR. Additionally, using a more stringent criterion to classify children at risk for mathematics difficulties resulted in a differential pattern of results when compared to a less stringent classification criterion.  相似文献   
906.
907.
学习型城市的理念从西方起源,并在欧洲进行了非常好的实践与传播,取得了非常多的研究和实践成果.近年来,学习型城市在欧洲的发展趋缓,发展的重点转向亚洲,并由独自发展转向合作发展.为促进学习型城市的可持续发展,各国都在制定发展策略以应对复杂的未来.学习型城市要进行理念创新,保障学习型城市发展中的"共同利益";进行合作创新,为学习型城市的国际合作创造各种机会;进行体系创新,从西方学习的理念与中国现状相结合.通过建立可持续的社会、经济和环境发展理念,学习型城市的未来发展才更加具有活力.  相似文献   
908.
909.
910.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号