首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1083篇
  免费   19篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   929篇
科学研究   28篇
各国文化   14篇
体育   32篇
文化理论   15篇
信息传播   85篇
  2023年   3篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   22篇
  2019年   29篇
  2018年   43篇
  2017年   35篇
  2016年   41篇
  2015年   21篇
  2014年   20篇
  2013年   287篇
  2012年   24篇
  2011年   27篇
  2010年   27篇
  2009年   27篇
  2008年   37篇
  2007年   18篇
  2006年   22篇
  2005年   31篇
  2004年   25篇
  2003年   18篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   19篇
  1999年   10篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   20篇
  1996年   28篇
  1995年   14篇
  1994年   11篇
  1993年   15篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   13篇
  1987年   9篇
  1986年   17篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   10篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   5篇
  1972年   3篇
  1923年   2篇
排序方式: 共有1103条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
An understanding of the uses of meta-cognition provides a powerful tool toward developing quality and efficiency in task learning and performance. Metacognition is the executive controller of cognitive processes responsible both for self regulated learning and work performance outcomes. Comprehension monitoring and motivation are two prime components of metacognition which are responsible for commitment to, and definition of, goals, monitoring progress toward goals, and activating the appropriate thinking skills to achieve goals. Specific thinking skill categories of attention, organization, and elaboration are described with examples of how specific techniques in each category can optimize learning and job performance. Research on training of metacognitive skills through both detached and embedded skill-development programs is reviewed. In addition, ways to compensate for metacognitive skills when training is not cost-effective are described. The relationship between metacognitive skills and effective management of work performance is stressed.  相似文献   
32.
33.
Abstract

We designed a survey to assess whether 40 randomly selected individuals, equally divided across two settings (ICF/MR, minigroup home), differed in the amounts of time spent in the community, the people with whom unstructured activities were performed, and the choice maker of unstructured activities performed in the community or in homes. Time spent in unstructured activities was divided into four subsets: performed by self, with a peer, with staff, and with family. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated that individuals living in ICF/MR homes spent more time in the community with staff and made fewer choices of their unstructured activities than those living in minigroup homes. The analyses also indicated that for those retaining their own legal guardianship and living in ICF/MR homes continued to spend less time in activities they chose themselves. Of those variables reaching statistical significance, we questioned meaningful interpretations based on alternative interpretations of the raw data.  相似文献   
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
Is religion as we interpret, teach and practice it today capable of motivating life? Can religion qualify or control conduct and so shape character? In common with lower forms of life, we respond to the drive of hunger, to the desire for a mate, to the compulsion of fear and anger, to the urge to live and to defend our lives. These motivations are all very simple and direct. They spring from nature's demand that the race shall not end, but shall go on.

But is there another source of motivation‐‐call it a higher source if you wil‐‐which is able to condition these more primitive and subjective drives and cause men to act with reference to values outside themselves? Is there a source of constraint capable of making men serve ideal ends? And is religion this source, has it this power over men?  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号