首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   110篇
  免费   2篇
教育   91篇
科学研究   7篇
体育   3篇
文化理论   1篇
信息传播   10篇
  2018年   2篇
  2017年   3篇
  2016年   4篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   2篇
  2013年   41篇
  2012年   1篇
  2011年   2篇
  2010年   1篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   5篇
  2007年   1篇
  2005年   3篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   1篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   1篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   2篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   1篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   2篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   1篇
  1971年   1篇
  1970年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
  1962年   1篇
  1958年   1篇
  1944年   4篇
  1941年   1篇
排序方式: 共有112条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
32.
33.
Journal of Science Education and Technology - This paper focuses on the notable heightening of underrepresented students’ engagement in STEM education through project-based learning CincySTEM...  相似文献   
34.
35.
T Sherman 《Child development》1985,56(6):1561-1573
The prototypical category-learning experiences with which infants are confronted are ones in which they have had prior experience with and have clearly noted the differences among a set of objects. Their task, then, is to notice also the similarities among the objects and to form a category based on these similarities. The goals of the current experiments were to create a laboratory situation in which it was clear that 10-month-old infants were distinguishing one category exemplar from another, and then to determine the structure of the category representation the infants developed under these learning conditions. A set of artificially created face stimuli was used in which the mean and modal prototypes were distinct. The infants' performance could not be explained by the context theory; they appear to have retained a count of the feature values observed during study. These data give support to the claim that prior to language, infants have available strategies for abstracting category-level information from their perceptual experiences with discriminable objects and events.  相似文献   
36.
37.
38.
ABSTRACT:  In this paper I explore the role of manners and morals.1 In particular, what is the connection between emotional demeanor and the inner stuff of virtue? Does the fact that we can pose faces and hide our inner sentiments, i.e., 'fake it,' detract from or add to our capacity for virtue? I argue, following a line from the Stoics, that it can add to our virtue and that, as a result, moral education needs to take seriously both a commitment to good character and a commitment to the 'aesthetic' of character.  相似文献   
39.
This paper describes a collaboration between an urban antipoverty agency, Action for Boston Community Development (ABCD), and a private higher education institution, Lesley College. This collaboration is providing a bachelor's degree program for adults associated with human service agencies. The introductory course, which is the focus of this paper, served to validate the students' experiential learning, helped them gain needed skills, and empowered them to make positive changes in both their perceptions and behaviors. Within a remarkable short period, the individuals in this group made significant changes in their lives. The program described can serve as a model in creating nontraditional partnerships to address the needs of underserved adult populations.  相似文献   
40.
Three teachers, who held extreme preferences for the ways they learn, participated in a year‐long professional development course, designed to sensitize teachers to their own and colleagues’ individual learning differences (ILDs). The case study focuses on their extreme learning preferences and discusses the impact of these preferences on their language, beliefs and practice, both before and after the course. The teachers’ learning preferences were determined from their scores on seven learning/cognitive styles tools and understood further from field notes, interviews and pre‐/post‐test responses. The study suggests that teachers with extreme learning preferences tend to: (a) teach the way they prefer to learn; (b) overgeneralize and project their own learning needs onto students; and (c) hold initial pathognomonic (“blame the learner”) beliefs about students mismatched to them. After the course, the teachers changed their language, beliefs and practice in the direction of becoming more effective teachers, e.g., they held more interventionist beliefs (“I can intervene to help the learner”). The three teachers were strong prototypes who can provide insights about the importance of ILDs in learning, practice and professional development.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号