首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   13741篇
  免费   186篇
  国内免费   11篇
教育   9412篇
科学研究   1830篇
各国文化   117篇
体育   992篇
综合类   17篇
文化理论   164篇
信息传播   1406篇
  2022年   97篇
  2021年   147篇
  2020年   208篇
  2019年   281篇
  2018年   415篇
  2017年   437篇
  2016年   390篇
  2015年   267篇
  2014年   379篇
  2013年   2201篇
  2012年   334篇
  2011年   319篇
  2010年   317篇
  2009年   276篇
  2008年   288篇
  2007年   296篇
  2006年   282篇
  2005年   241篇
  2004年   268篇
  2003年   229篇
  2002年   246篇
  2001年   325篇
  2000年   343篇
  1999年   254篇
  1998年   142篇
  1997年   187篇
  1996年   182篇
  1995年   122篇
  1994年   139篇
  1993年   122篇
  1992年   206篇
  1991年   187篇
  1990年   215篇
  1989年   211篇
  1988年   183篇
  1987年   193篇
  1986年   174篇
  1985年   209篇
  1984年   154篇
  1983年   134篇
  1982年   127篇
  1981年   109篇
  1980年   114篇
  1979年   176篇
  1978年   132篇
  1977年   115篇
  1976年   91篇
  1975年   94篇
  1974年   95篇
  1973年   91篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
In an attempt to clarify the chronological relationship between identity and equivalence conservation, normal preschool-aged children (experiment 1) were exposed to brief videotape demonstrations of a model conserving identity and equivalence, identity only, equivalence only, or neither. Subsequent performances indicate that identity was easier to accelerate than equivalence, and training in both identity and equivalence appeared to be most effective in accelerating conservation. Trainable mentally retarded (TMR) children were then exposed to either the identity-equivalence-conserving or control model (experiment 2). This population, which some have argued remains at a preoperational level of functioning, was more likely to conserve identity than equivalence. In addition, conservation in the training group increased following training, though these gains were not maintained over a 3-week retention interval.  相似文献   
53.
First-grade males performed a 30-min visual vigilance task under 5 conditions of auditory background stimulation. The 5 conditions consisted of (1) continuous conversation, in which subjects listened to a tape of conversation spliced so that there were no intervals of silence lasting longer than 3 sec; (2) continuous reverse, in which subjects heard the continuous conversation tape played in reverse; (3) intermittent conversation, in which subjects heard alternating segments of conversation and silence; (4) intermittent reverse, in which subjects heard alternating segments of reverse conversation and silence; and (5) silence control. Compared with silence or continuous stimulation, intermittent stimulation produced better detection regardless of whether or not it was meaningful. High achievers made more correct detections than low achievers, but only in the second and third time periods. Few subjects made errors of commission.  相似文献   
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
60.
The techniques described in Part I of this paper for the calculation of plastic failure loads under proportional loading are further exemplified by indicating solutions for a rectangular building frame, a Vierendeel girder, and a shed-type portal frame. A method for estimating deflections of a frame just as the collapse load is attained is then described, with some indications as to its reliability. More general conditions of loading than proportional loading are next discussed, and generalized load and failure concepts are defined. The theorems and techniques for determining critical shakedown loads under variable, repeated loading are then described and illustrated. The relation of the elastic-plastic shakedown analysis to the much simpler analysis for failure under single load applications is discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号