首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   799篇
  免费   4篇
教育   620篇
科学研究   40篇
各国文化   5篇
体育   47篇
文化理论   10篇
信息传播   81篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   13篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   21篇
  2019年   24篇
  2018年   29篇
  2017年   24篇
  2016年   30篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   32篇
  2013年   211篇
  2012年   26篇
  2011年   36篇
  2010年   40篇
  2009年   25篇
  2008年   22篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   11篇
  2005年   17篇
  2004年   14篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   16篇
  2001年   12篇
  2000年   15篇
  1999年   14篇
  1998年   12篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   12篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   3篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   2篇
  1982年   2篇
  1976年   4篇
  1974年   3篇
  1971年   3篇
  1969年   2篇
  1968年   3篇
  1966年   2篇
  1900年   2篇
  1847年   1篇
  1844年   1篇
  1833年   1篇
排序方式: 共有803条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
As regular readers know, I have been lucky enough to travel to many parts of China. What you may not know is that I am, mostly, a people person, that is, someone who is more interested in people than other things. With that in mind, I want to tell you about my visit to one of China's wonders, the Terracotta Soldiers near Xi'an.  相似文献   
102.
六年前的某一天,Brad在北京街头等车。因为语言不通而略显困惑。一位年轻女士的一句“May I help you?”。一段友谊由此产生……  相似文献   
103.
竹茱 《英语沙龙》2010,(3):37-37
当人们谈论起美洲大陆时,总会说:"哥伦布是第一个发现美洲大陆的人。"如今在加拿大的学校中,老师也是这样教授学生们的。但事实果真如此吗?我们都知道,明朝年间,朱棣派郑和帅舰队下西洋,他们向西航行到了很远的地方,并在航行期间绘制了航行的海图和地图。后来,哥伦布发现新大陆时,使用的就是郑和他们当年绘制的地图。但遗憾的是,由于各种原因,国内档案中几乎没有留下能说明郑和发现美洲大陆的史料。那么,对于美洲大陆的发现是要归功于郑和还是哥伦布呢?  相似文献   
104.
Liu Bin刚刚跨入了大学校门。大学里的生活自由灵活。比起高中备受约束的日子可谓天壤之别,可这没有老师“管教”的生活。Liubjn反而有点不适应了。如何安排好自己的大学生活?Brad又会有哪些建议?  相似文献   
105.
A growing body of research incorporates children’s perspectives into the research process. If we are to take children’s perspectives seriously in education research, research methodologies must be capable of addressing issues that matter to children. This article engages in a theoretical discussion that considers how a posthuman research methodology can support such an effort. Piaget’s early and lesser known qualitative studies on children’s conception of the world are re-read along with Karen Barad’s posthuman theory, using Catherine Malabou’s concept of plasticity. Through a plastic reading of Piaget and Barad, I consider how a posthuman theoretical framework might contribute to research seeking to access children’s perspectives. Before concluding, I reflect on some ethical concerns regarding posthuman research in education.  相似文献   
106.
Abstract

College men and women were tested as to reaction time and speed of an arm movement using both motor-oriented and stimulus-oriented set. The results confirmed a hypothesis based on neuromotor coordination theory that predicted slower movement and greater reaction latency when the motor set was used. However, the 20 percent of subjects who had a natural motor set tendency moved faster with an enforced motor set than with an enforced sensory set. The conditions of enforced set caused a moderate positive correlation between reaction and movement times. Women subjects reacted and moved slower than men, but were similarly influenced by the two enforced set conditions. Their natural set tendency was definitely stimulus-oriented, while men tended to have a neutral orientation.  相似文献   
107.
A feminist action research team, which consisted of a science educator, an English‐language learner (ELL) educator, a first‐year science teacher, and a graduate assistant, set a goal to work together to explore the process a beginning teacher goes through to establish a classroom conducive to the needs of middle‐level ELL learners. The guiding questions of the study were answered by gathering a wealth of data over the course of 5 months and taken from the classroom, planning sessions, and researchers and students. These data were collected by observations, semistructured interviews, and written document reviews. The progressive analysis ultimately revealed that: (a) successful strategies a beginning teacher must utilize for teaching middle‐level ELL children in a mainstream classroom involve complex structural considerations that are not part of the teacher's preparation; (b) learning increases for all children, but there are differences in learning achievement between ELL and non‐ELL children; and (c) student and peer feedback proved to be an effective means of enhancing the growth of a beginning teacher seeking to increase her skills in teaching ELL learners. The experiences and findings from this project have implications for teacher preparation programs committed to preparing educators to teach science to all children. © 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Res Sci Teach 42: 1013–1031, 2005  相似文献   
108.
Cupples  Linda  Iacono  Teresa 《Reading and writing》2002,15(5-6):549-574
An intervention study was conducted toinvestigate whether children with Down syndrome(DS) would benefit from an `analytic' approachto reading instruction, which encompassedexplicit training in phonological awareness.Participants were seven English-speakingchildren with DS aged 8;6 (years;months) to11;1, who demonstrated little or nononword-reading ability prior to intervention.The children received weekly instruction (forsix weeks) in reading aloud 30 regularly speltmonosyllables (e.g., ten, bake) using an`analytic' approach, in which words werelearned by combining onsets with rimes (fourchildren), or a `whole-word' approach (threechildren). Participants' oral reading wasassessed pre- and post-intervention using areading test comprising the 30 trained wordsand 30 untrained (generalisation) words. Mostchildren (six out of seven) read more trainingwords correctly after intervention than before,with significant improvement shown by fourchildren (two trained analytically, and twotrained with whole words). More importantly,reading of generalisation words improvedsignificantly in only three children, all ofwhom had received analytic training. It wasconcluded that children with DS benefit from ananalytic approach to reading instruction, eventhough their auditory-verbal memory (assessedusing digit span) is poor.  相似文献   
109.
Reviews involving the Wechsler Scales for children suggest that Full Scale IQ scores on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition, average 5 to 6 points lower than scores on the second edition of the scale [WISC‐R, Wechsler, D. (1974). Zimmerman & Woo‐Sam, 1997], with the differences distributed disproportionately over subtests, i.e., with larger discrepancies found within the Performance Scale (Wechsler, 1991). Changes on the revised subtests of the WISC‐III Performance Scale may place children with ADHD at a disadvantage compared to their performance on analogous WISC‐R subtests. We examined IQ test performance in 122 unmedicated children with ADHD (61 given the WISC‐R, 61 given the WISC‐III), and 46 children from a healthy, comparison group (23 given the WISC‐R, 23 given the WISC‐III). The ADHD and comparison group samples were matched for sex and for Verbal IQ between WISC‐R and WISC‐III. Children with ADHD had significantly lower Performance IQ on WISC‐III compared to the WISC‐R, with the Picture Arrangement subtest showing the most significant difference. In contrast, there were no significant differences between the WISC‐R and WISC‐III cohorts on Performance IQ or any Performance subtests among the comparison group. These findings highlight the importance of examining the comparability of ability test revisions among clinical and non‐clinical populations, and will be especially salient when the WISC‐III is revised. © 2003 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Psychol Schs 40: 331–340, 2003.  相似文献   
110.
One hundred thirty-two graduates with learning disabilities (LD) of a large, public, competitive postsecondary institution were surveyed to determine if they had self-disclosed their LD to their current employer and to provide the reasons for choosing to self-disclose or not to self-disclose. Based on a response rate of 67.4% (n = 89), the results indicated that 86.5% of the respondents were employed full time. Although nearly 90% of the respondents stated that their LD affected their work in some way, only 30.3% self-disclosed to their employer. Of those who had not self-disclosed, the majority reported that there was no reason or need to self-disclose. However, 46.1% reported not self-disclosing due to fear of a potentially negative impact in the workplace or due to a concern for job security. Specific rationales for disclosure and information related to the use of self-reported accommodations and strategies are presented.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号