首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2172篇
  免费   39篇
教育   1552篇
科学研究   144篇
各国文化   19篇
体育   218篇
文化理论   17篇
信息传播   261篇
  2022年   11篇
  2021年   14篇
  2020年   36篇
  2019年   74篇
  2018年   96篇
  2017年   89篇
  2016年   72篇
  2015年   40篇
  2014年   59篇
  2013年   510篇
  2012年   47篇
  2011年   46篇
  2010年   32篇
  2009年   42篇
  2008年   47篇
  2007年   37篇
  2006年   55篇
  2005年   31篇
  2004年   48篇
  2003年   38篇
  2002年   43篇
  2001年   32篇
  2000年   44篇
  1999年   38篇
  1998年   24篇
  1997年   26篇
  1996年   15篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   27篇
  1993年   19篇
  1992年   22篇
  1991年   26篇
  1990年   17篇
  1989年   24篇
  1988年   21篇
  1987年   16篇
  1986年   15篇
  1985年   20篇
  1984年   25篇
  1983年   18篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   25篇
  1977年   12篇
  1974年   14篇
  1972年   9篇
  1971年   10篇
  1927年   8篇
  1926年   10篇
排序方式: 共有2211条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
Unattended science and technology exhibits of both static and operational types have been an integral part of museum displays for many years. More recently interactive exhibits in which observers are encouraged to become part of the system of exhibits have become more common. A study was commenced to explore the impact and potential of low cost, unattended, interactive exhibitsset up singly in a normal school classroom without the distractions of a multiplicity of activities as is common in ‘science museums’. Three small groups of Grade 5/6 primary school children interacted with a ‘Falling Towers’ exhibit and their voluntary activities were recorded on videotape for later analysis. Children appeared to state the results of their activity in ways consistent with their expectations rather than with their most recent experience with the exhibit. The responses of girls, boys and mixed groups are reported. Specializations: primary mathematics and science education, teaching strategies. Specializations: science education, students' understandings of phenomena in science.  相似文献   
962.
Seeing things differently: teachers' constructions of underachievement   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The identity of the underachiever has become synonymous with the stereotypical identity of boys. Teachers know what underachievement looks like: it looks like a boy who is bright, but bored. Evidence from a research study reported here demonstrates that teachers are more likely to select boys as underachievers than girls and that teachers construct underachievement differentially by gender. The consequence is that underachievement in girls is often overlooked or rendered invisible. Underachievement is concerned with potential not lack of ability, while high and low achievement are concerned with performance. It becomes a matter of concern if teachers perceive boys as the vessel of potential and of latent ability, while the high achievements of girls are seen to be about performance, not ability.  相似文献   
963.
The purpose of this study was to examine the moderating role of individual differences in negative emotionality in the relations of behavioral and attentional (emotional) regulation to externalizing problem behaviors. Teachers' and one parent's reports of children's regulation (attentional and behavioral), emotionality, and problem behavior were obtained when children were in kindergarten to grade 3 and two years later (N = 169; 146 in major analyses); children's behavioral regulation also was assessed with a measure of persistence. According to the best fitting structural equation model, at two ages behavioral dysregulation predicted externalizing behavior problems for children both high and low in negative emotionality, whereas prediction of problem behavior from attentional control was significant only for children prone to negative emotionality. There were unique, additive effects of behavioral and attentional regulation for predicting problem behavior as well as moderating effects of negative emotionality for attentional regulation.  相似文献   
964.
965.
966.
967.
968.
969.
970.
Strength training with isometric contractions produces large but highly angle-specific adaptations. To contrast the contractile mode of isometric versus dynamic training, but diminish the strong angle specificity effect, we compared the strength gains produced by isometric training at four joint angles with conventional dynamic training. Thirty-three recreationally active healthy males aged 18 - 30 years completed 9 weeks of strength training of the quadriceps muscle group three times per week. An intra-individual design was adopted: one leg performed purely isometric training at each of four joint angles (isometrically trained leg); the other leg performed conventional dynamic training, lifting and lowering (dynamically trained leg). Both legs trained at similar relative loads for the same duration. The quadriceps strength of each leg was measured isometrically (at four angles) and isokinetically (at three velocities) pre and post training. After 9 weeks of training, the increase in isokinetic strength was similar in both legs (pooled data from three velocities: dynamically trained leg, 10.7%; isometrically trained leg, 10.5%). Isometric strength increases were significantly greater for the isometrically trained leg (pooled data from four angles: dynamically trained leg, 13.1%; isometrically trained leg, 18.0%). This may have been due to the greater absolute torque involved with isometric training or a residual angle specificity effect despite the isometric training being divided over four angles.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号