首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   23390篇
  免费   269篇
  国内免费   21篇
教育   15607篇
科学研究   3602篇
各国文化   194篇
体育   1675篇
综合类   21篇
文化理论   231篇
信息传播   2350篇
  2021年   223篇
  2020年   326篇
  2019年   458篇
  2018年   637篇
  2017年   641篇
  2016年   557篇
  2015年   386篇
  2014年   540篇
  2013年   3974篇
  2012年   524篇
  2011年   486篇
  2010年   446篇
  2009年   402篇
  2008年   433篇
  2007年   452篇
  2006年   388篇
  2005年   362篇
  2004年   375篇
  2003年   343篇
  2002年   354篇
  2001年   450篇
  2000年   490篇
  1999年   382篇
  1998年   212篇
  1997年   261篇
  1996年   267篇
  1995年   192篇
  1994年   216篇
  1993年   201篇
  1992年   315篇
  1991年   309篇
  1990年   333篇
  1989年   347篇
  1988年   287篇
  1987年   290篇
  1986年   313篇
  1985年   333篇
  1984年   267篇
  1983年   239篇
  1982年   229篇
  1981年   192篇
  1980年   204篇
  1979年   292篇
  1978年   242篇
  1977年   209篇
  1976年   183篇
  1975年   164篇
  1974年   195篇
  1973年   175篇
  1971年   181篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
982.
The evaluation of faculty development programs   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
This article reviews literature pertinent to the evaluation of faculty development programs and presents data from several studies conducted at two institutions. These data were consistent with those previously reported in that faculty participants consistently expressed satisfaction with development services. In addition, one study found that most faculty members voluntarily took some action to improve their instructional effectiveness, though only a minority pursued these efforts in depth. Volunteers who worked intensively with a faculty development consultant improved more on objective measures of effectiveness than did those who were only superficially involved in improvement efforts; those who received no consultative assistance failed to improve significantly. Evidence from a final study provided a control for faculty motivation and led to the conclusion that improvement was contingent both on faculty desire to improve and on the availability of professional assistance.  相似文献   
983.
984.
985.
One hundred eighty-one experienced teachers (elementary and junior high), and 189 preservice teachers were surveyed regarding their perceptions of school psychologists. The survey instrument used essentially the same categories as Styles in 1965, including: (a) level of training, (b) effectiveness, (c) qualifications for tasks, and (d) usefulness in specific duties. Analysis of the survey data showed there were significant differences between preservice and experienced teachers' effectiveness ratings of school psychologists on eight tasks typically performed by school psychologists. Significant differences also were found between preservice and experienced teachers on their ratings of school psychologists' qualifications to undertake specific tasks, although almost half of the experienced teachers were unable to rate the effectiveness of school psychologists. There was a small, but significant, negative relationship between the amount of contact with the school psychologist and the perceived effectiveness. Limitations inherent in survey methodology and the need to foster greater teacher awareness of the school psychologist's role are discussed.  相似文献   
986.
987.
988.
Sequence learning in pigeons was studied in asimultaneous chaining paradigm: all stimuli and the opportunity to respond to each stimulus were available simultaneously. In contrast to the traditionalsuccessive chaining paradigm, a simultaneous chaining paradigm provides no differential feedback following each response (except the last). Subjects were first trained to perform on sequences of two (AB), then three (ABC), and then four colors (ABCD). Performance greatly exceeded that predicted by models of random choice. Generalization to novel arrays of three and four colors was complete. After training with a four-color sequence, the subjects were tested with subsequences consisting of all possible combinations of two and three of the four training colors (e.g., BD, AD, BC, ACD, BCD, etc.). The successful completion of these subsequences showed that the organization of the original sequence did not entail overt pecks to successive elements of that sequence. That subjects can respond accurately on nonadjacent subsets is not readily explained by a chaining theory, or by any theory that assumes that responding to element n provides a cue for responding to element n+1.  相似文献   
989.
A task involving simple mathematics, yet complex in its call for the generation of multiple solution methods, was administered to about 150 U.S. students, most of whom were in fourth grade. Written responses were examined for correctness, evidence of strategy use and mode of explanation. Results for the U.S. sample were also compared to those obtained from about 200 Japanese fourth-grade students. Students in both countries (a) produced multiple solutions and explanations of their solutions, (b) exhibited almost identical patterns and frequency of strategy use across response occasions, and (c) used the same kinds of explanations, with a majority of the responses involving solution explanations that combined both visual and verbal/symbolic features. Nevertheless, Japanese students tended to produce explanations involving more sophisticated mathematical ideas (multiplication rather than addition) and formalisms (mathematical expressions rather than verbal explanations) than did U.S. students.  相似文献   
990.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号