首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   344篇
  免费   9篇
教育   277篇
科学研究   32篇
各国文化   2篇
体育   7篇
文化理论   5篇
信息传播   30篇
  2023年   3篇
  2021年   3篇
  2019年   3篇
  2018年   11篇
  2017年   4篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   97篇
  2012年   8篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   6篇
  2009年   10篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   8篇
  2006年   4篇
  2005年   7篇
  2004年   10篇
  2003年   4篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   6篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   6篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   3篇
  1987年   3篇
  1985年   5篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   8篇
  1978年   3篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   4篇
  1971年   2篇
  1967年   2篇
  1966年   3篇
  1960年   3篇
  1940年   2篇
  1935年   2篇
  1930年   2篇
  1928年   3篇
  1867年   3篇
排序方式: 共有353条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
271.
This interpretative research portrays the turbulent conditions that are typical of my experiences as a science supervisor in the central office of a large urban school district in southeastern United States implementing science systemic reform. The research documents a dependence on an authoritarian view of policy-making that combined with procedures for making assignments allows stakeholders to undervalue the decisions that they make and increases the possibility for blaming others for their decisions. The impact of traditional and reform attitudes about curriculum and schools is described as well as efforts to resolve these conflicting views in the process of developing a middle school curriculum guide. The frustrating effect of the interactions of changing curriculum guides, changing instructional materials, and insufficient resources are described in a visit to a middle school science department meeting. The roles of time, understanding of systemic assumptions, and appropriate inquiry and communication skills are critical to the decision making process and little attention is given to address these issues. The result is that decisions end up being made without resolution of the differences among stakeholders leading to negative feelings and creating currents of frustration and distrust that undermine reform efforts.  相似文献   
272.
A model for thinking about training evaluation is proposed. The model consists of context and process factors on the one hand, and outcome factors on the other. Among context and process factors are basis of training, purpose of training, unit of productivity, data sources, kind of training, who is trained, and costs of training. Outcome factors include employee, customer, and corporate aspects. Assumptions of the model include a) that both corporatist and humanistic conceptions of training are to be viewed as valid, and b) that the term training is constrictive and probably should be replaced by the term education, which is more elastic. An impetus for the model is that work and workplaces are changing. How knowledge, skills, and orientations toward evaluation are utilized must be viewed somewhat more expansively than might have been appropriate under work conditions in “Tayloristic” times.  相似文献   
273.
A method of producing a cam which will always be in two-point contact with an oscillating follower is developed in comparatively general terms in connection with the design of a pump. The contacting portions of the follower are, however, limited to circular arcs of equal radius. A method of computing ideal rates of flow is discussed.  相似文献   
274.
275.
276.
277.
278.
In Phase 1 of the study (1991–1992 school year), 43 elementary school students in five special education classes for behaviorally disordered children were classified using the Differential Test of Conduct and Emotional Problems. Subjects were classified as primarily emotional (ED) or conduct problems (CP). Results were as follows: 11 ED (26%), 15 CP (35%), 13 both ED and CP (31%), and 4 (9%) falling into neither grouping. During Phase 2 (1996–1997 school year), a different cohort of students (n = 68), from the same five elementary special education classes plus one junior high school class, was also classified on the Learning Style Identification Scale. Phase 2 results were 12 students ED (18%), 16 CP (24%), 14 both ED and CP (21%), and 26 neither (38%). Eighty‐eight percent of conduct problem students were classified as Style 1 learners. Findings are examined in light of federal special education eligibility criteria, specifically the exclusion of “socially maladjusted” students. Implications for the multidisciplinary team placement process and educational programming are discussed. © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.  相似文献   
279.
We argue in this article that the discrepancy model of needs assessment is lacking in that it does not capture expert behavior, and hence can not lead to problem–solving expertise. With arguments based on a review of the theory and research literature related to problem solving and reflections based on practical experience, we contend that in the first instance, organizational problems are typically too illdefined and shrouded in ambiguity to be meaningfully captured by the model. In the real world, current and desired states cannot always be clearly articulated. We review literature showing that problem–solving expertise, while having a process component, is premised on possession of domain–specific knowledge. Expertise is knowledge based. It comes from wide and varied experience, based on learned principles and rules, which leads to problem categorization, to automacity, and to the deliberate selection or formulation of strategies. The expert draws on schemata stored in long–term memory. Thus we contend that by dwelling almost exclusively on the discrepancy approach to problem solving, the field of Human Resource Development unwittingly forfeits the opportunity to engage in a discourse which would be premised on expert practice.  相似文献   
280.
Students' employment of meaning through cognitive strategies in a retention task was explored in terms of attributional assignment and/or divergent thinking. Students were randomly assigned to either a list of nonsense syllables or nonrelated words. Attributional assignment was measured by Lefcourt's Multidimensional-Multiattributional Causality Scales (MMCS – IV); divergent thinking was measured by Guilford's tests of Fluency, Flexibility, and Originality. A questionnaire assessing students' beliefs regarding success and failure was also administered. No relationship was found among attributional assignment as measured by the MMCS-IV, divergent thinking, and retention. Strategy use was not related to retention. Students' attributional beliefs regarding failure were predictive of retention for the nonsense syllables. Discussion suggests the need to distinguish between tactic and strategy through analysis of metacognitive processes. Interpretation of the results may indicate that the divergent-thinking measures were not sensitive enough and/or the retention task did not require these divergent-thinking skills.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号