首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   14860篇
  免费   241篇
  国内免费   11篇
教育   10376篇
科学研究   1742篇
各国文化   118篇
体育   1183篇
综合类   18篇
文化理论   193篇
信息传播   1482篇
  2022年   120篇
  2021年   171篇
  2020年   250篇
  2019年   372篇
  2018年   524篇
  2017年   528篇
  2016年   496篇
  2015年   306篇
  2014年   434篇
  2013年   2461篇
  2012年   374篇
  2011年   341篇
  2010年   347篇
  2009年   293篇
  2008年   304篇
  2007年   311篇
  2006年   293篇
  2005年   247篇
  2004年   277篇
  2003年   243篇
  2002年   255篇
  2001年   330篇
  2000年   341篇
  1999年   260篇
  1998年   147篇
  1997年   191篇
  1996年   184篇
  1995年   129篇
  1994年   142篇
  1993年   123篇
  1992年   205篇
  1991年   188篇
  1990年   220篇
  1989年   217篇
  1988年   186篇
  1987年   198篇
  1986年   175篇
  1985年   219篇
  1984年   163篇
  1983年   133篇
  1982年   131篇
  1981年   113篇
  1980年   117篇
  1979年   180篇
  1978年   138篇
  1977年   114篇
  1976年   92篇
  1975年   94篇
  1974年   99篇
  1973年   100篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 203 毫秒
81.
This study explores science communication on Twitter by investigating a sample of tweets referring to academic papers in five different scientific fields. The specifications of science communicators on Twitter, the characteristics of those who initiate actions (by tweeting), the extent and quality of reactions (retweeting), individual and group interactions, and the distribution of tweets across types of engagement in the process of science communication (i.e., dissemination, consultation, and evaluation) were explored. A broad array of actors is involved in the communication of science on Twitter, with individual citizens and individual researchers playing an important role. In principle, this is promising for creating direct interaction, which can be difficult through more traditional mass media. The vast majority of communication activities regarding academic papers is undigested dissemination with almost no sign of debate, contestation, or collective reflection. Another general finding of this study is that bot accounts play a major role in the science communication landscape on Twitter.  相似文献   
82.
83.
84.
85.
In collaborative problem solving, children produce and evaluate arguments for proposals. We investigated whether 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 192) can produce and evaluate arguments against those arguments (i.e., counter-arguments). In Study 1, each child within a peer dyad was privately given a reason to prefer one over another solution to a task. One child, however, was given further information that would refute the reasoning of their partner. Five-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, identified and produced valid and relevant counter-arguments. In Study 2, 3-year-olds were given discourse training (discourse that contrasted valid and invalid counter-arguments) and then given the same problem-solving tasks. After training, 3-year-olds could also identify and produce valid and relevant counter-arguments. Thus, participating in discourse about reasons facilitates children’s counter-argumentation.  相似文献   
86.
87.
88.
In several of his papers, Charles S. Peirce illustrates processes of interpreting and understanding signs by examples from second language vocabulary teaching and learning. The insights conveyed by means of these little pedagogical scenarios are not meant as contributions to the psychology of second language learning, but they aim at elucidating fundamental semiotic implications of knowledge acquisition in general. Peirce's semiotic premise that a well‐understood sign is one that represents an object and creates an interpretant is essential to the understanding of how new words and signs in general can be taught and learned. The article argues that Peirce's theory of the object of the sign, especially of the necessity of collateral experience of the object of a sign, can help to understand the riddle posed by of the Meno paradox of the impossibility of learning what we do not yet know. It examines the semiotic implications of the didactic methods of teaching and learning through translation, ostension, mental and real images, as well as metacognition, and it shows how icons, indices, and symbols are essential to learning new words.  相似文献   
89.
90.
In the article we solve path planning task for an agent being multirotor unmanned aerial vehicle (multicopter). We propose an approach of estimating path geometry constraints based on UAV flight dynamics model and control constraints. Than we introduce a new path finding method which takes into consideration those geometry constraints and study this method both theoretically and empirically.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号