首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   318篇
  免费   6篇
教育   258篇
科学研究   14篇
各国文化   4篇
体育   32篇
文化理论   1篇
信息传播   15篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   12篇
  2017年   9篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   8篇
  2013年   79篇
  2012年   3篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   6篇
  2009年   8篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   5篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   6篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   6篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   4篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   6篇
  1973年   3篇
  1971年   3篇
  1970年   4篇
  1969年   2篇
  1967年   2篇
  1966年   2篇
  1964年   3篇
排序方式: 共有324条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
301.
302.
303.
304.
This is a cross-sectional study of 519 undergraduate engineering majors’ self-efficacy beliefs at a large, research extensive, Midwestern university. Engineering self-efficacy is an individual’s belief in his or her ability to successfully negotiate the academic hurdles of the engineering program. Engineering self-efficacy was obtained from four variables: self-efficacy 1, self-efficacy 2, engineering career outcome expectations, and coping self-efficacy. The four variables were analyzed using a repeated analysis of variance among levels of gender, ethnicity, years students had been enrolled in their engineering program, and transfer status. No significant differences in mean engineering self-efficacy scores were found by gender, ethnicity, and transfer status. However, significant interactions between gender and the subscales, ethnicity and the subscales, and transfer status and the subscales were found. Significant differences in mean engineering self-efficacy scores were found among years students had been enrolled in the program.  相似文献   
305.
Theory of mind requires an understanding of both desires and beliefs. Moreover, children understand desires before beliefs. Little is known about the mechanisms underlying this developmental lag. Additionally, previous neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies have neglected the direct comparison of these developmentally critical mental-state concepts. Event-related brain potentials were recorded as participants ( N  =   24; mean age   =   22 years) reasoned about diverse-desires, diverse-beliefs, and parallel physical situations. A mid-frontal late slow wave (LSW) was associated with desire and belief judgments. A right-posterior LSW was only associated with belief judgments. These findings demonstrate neural overlap and critical differences in reasoning explicitly about desires and beliefs, and they suggest children recruit additional neural processes for belief judgments beyond a common, more general, mentalizing neural system.  相似文献   
306.
Classic theories of self-development suggest people define themselves in part through internalized perceptions of other people's beliefs about them, known as reflected self-appraisals. This study uses functional magnetic resonance imaging to compare the neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescence ( N  = 12, ages 11–14 years) and adulthood ( N  = 12, ages 23–30 years). During direct self-reflection, adolescents demonstrated greater activity than adults in networks relevant to self-perception (medial prefrontal and parietal cortices) and social-cognition (dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, temporal–parietal junction, and posterior superior temporal sulcus), suggesting adolescent self-construals may rely more heavily on others' perspectives about the self. Activity in the medial fronto-parietal network was also enhanced when adolescents took the perspective of someone more relevant to a given domain.  相似文献   
307.
308.
309.
310.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号