首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1409篇
  免费   30篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   1024篇
科学研究   123篇
各国文化   31篇
体育   116篇
综合类   1篇
文化理论   10篇
信息传播   135篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   22篇
  2020年   31篇
  2019年   48篇
  2018年   69篇
  2017年   70篇
  2016年   61篇
  2015年   29篇
  2014年   38篇
  2013年   280篇
  2012年   35篇
  2011年   55篇
  2010年   38篇
  2009年   34篇
  2008年   37篇
  2007年   29篇
  2006年   33篇
  2005年   16篇
  2004年   24篇
  2003年   21篇
  2002年   16篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   26篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   15篇
  1996年   16篇
  1995年   20篇
  1994年   20篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   19篇
  1991年   15篇
  1990年   18篇
  1989年   11篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   11篇
  1984年   15篇
  1983年   12篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   14篇
  1980年   8篇
  1978年   10篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   6篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   5篇
  1927年   6篇
  1923年   6篇
排序方式: 共有1440条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
半年不见的Brad又回来了!喜欢旅行和爱好广泛的他,似乎永远都会为我们带来不同的精彩故事和见闻。那让我们依旧跟随他的文字和图片,穿越空间上的距离,一同享受那游走在世界的美丽心情!  相似文献   
152.
153.
OBJECTIVE: Our studies compared individuals at high- and low-risk for child physical abuse on measures of social information processing. METHOD: Two studies were conducted using similar methods. Twenty-eight childless women in Study 1 and 36 mothers in Study 2 read vignettes of parent-child interactions in which the child's level of compliance was difficult to interpret. Participants were asked a series of questions about the child's behavior and their own reactions. RESULTS: Accuracy and bias in identifying compliant behavior were assessed using a signal detection paradigm. In both samples, high- and low-risk participants did not differ in their overall accuracy in identifying children's behaviors. However, they used different evaluation standards such that high-risk participants were biased toward seeing more noncompliance and low-risk participants were biased toward seeing more compliance. High- and low-risk participants also made different types of errors in interpreting children's behavior. Low-risk participants were more likely to misinterpret noncompliant behavior as compliant, and there was a trend for high-risk participants to not perceive compliant behavior when it occurred. There were no differences in reported disciplinary responses in either study and the results for affective reactions were mixed. CONCLUSIONS: Specific differences in social information processing between high- and low-risk individuals replicated across samples, suggesting a reliable association between evaluation standards and risk of child physical abuse. However, the absence of differences in reported discipline and inconsistent findings on affective reactions indicate the need to identify the mechanism through which cognition influences parenting behavior.  相似文献   
154.
155.
156.
157.
158.
Abstract

This yearlong study explores how digital technologies are used during literacy instruction in the critical year of third grade. We analyzed technology use in 16 classrooms across six geographically and socioeconomically diverse schools. We examined the multiple layers of technology’s influences on teachers’ instructional decision-making and on students’ engagement. Applying Bloom’s Revised Taxonomy alongside the SAMR model, expectations for student technology use varied across settings and resulted in mixed levels of student engagement. Although technology has the potential to transform teaching and learning, it is most often used as a substitution for traditional instructional tools and to support students’ remembering and understanding rather than more complex tasks. Analysis suggests technology tools are not being strategically employed to support the development of 21st century learning skills.  相似文献   
159.
This longitudinal study evaluated the extent to which maternal responsiveness across early childhood and children's cognitive skills predicted children's 8-year decoding and reading comprehension skills for children who varied in biological risk (term, n = 83; preterm, n = 155). Patterns of maternal responsiveness during infancy (6, 12, and 24 months) and preschool (3 and 4 years) revealed 4 maternal clusters that varied in consistency and level of maternal responsiveness. Although not predictive of decoding skills, the interaction between children's 4-year cognitive ability and maternal responsiveness cluster predicted children's reading comprehension skills at 8 years of age, regardless of risk. Although consistently high levels of maternal responsive parenting across early childhood related to literacy outcomes for all children in the study, responsive parenting had a stronger relation to later reading comprehension skills for children with lower cognitive abilities, particularly when mothers demonstrated high responsiveness in children's infancy.  相似文献   
160.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号