首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1603篇
  免费   39篇
  国内免费   5篇
教育   932篇
科学研究   189篇
各国文化   9篇
体育   375篇
综合类   11篇
文化理论   6篇
信息传播   125篇
  2024年   5篇
  2023年   5篇
  2022年   4篇
  2020年   6篇
  2017年   3篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   14篇
  2012年   103篇
  2011年   170篇
  2010年   43篇
  2009年   17篇
  2008年   101篇
  2007年   126篇
  2006年   109篇
  2005年   113篇
  2004年   79篇
  2003年   94篇
  2002年   96篇
  2001年   55篇
  2000年   71篇
  1999年   36篇
  1998年   16篇
  1997年   19篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   17篇
  1992年   31篇
  1991年   24篇
  1990年   29篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   11篇
  1987年   16篇
  1986年   21篇
  1985年   30篇
  1984年   24篇
  1983年   16篇
  1982年   15篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   6篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   3篇
  1973年   2篇
  1959年   6篇
  1958年   4篇
  1957年   4篇
  1955年   3篇
  1938年   2篇
排序方式: 共有1647条查询结果,搜索用时 78 毫秒
131.
132.
任何一个复杂的问题都是近乎无数的事实和关系的组合。商业行为尤其受到方方面面的影响,包括过去的、非逻辑的和不可知的事物的影响。这种复杂性又因目标不同与服务于各种各样的顾客的需要而更加盘根错节,而顾客的许多目标不得不经过权衡后放弃。在这样复杂的条件下解决问题需要井井有条的、系统的方法以试图优化最后的决定。  相似文献   
133.
类萜烯化合物是在许多种香精油,特别是针叶树香精油中所发现的烃类(C_(10)H_1)之一。这里,三位在地理上远隔的实验室中工作的西班牙化学家,完全用技术词汇叙述了他们在转变和合成各种类萜烯化合物方面的工作成果。小的发展中国家的研究小组可以从这篇报道中得到鼓舞。  相似文献   
134.
135.
脑的性别差异   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
136.
Abrams D 《Child development》2011,82(5):1579-1593
Does children’s bias toward their own groups reflect egocentrism or social understanding? After being categorized as belonging to 1 of 2 fictitious groups, 157 six‐ to ten‐year‐olds evaluated group members and expressed preferences among neutral items. Children who expected the in‐group to share their item preferences (egocentric social projection) showed intergroup bias. However, most bias was expressed by children who expected their in‐group to share, but the out‐group to oppose, their own evaluations of members. These oppositional expectations were associated with better social perspective taking, and better understanding that groups expect loyalty from their members. Consistent with the developmental model of subjective group dynamics (D. Abrams, A. Rutland, J. Pelletier, & J. M. Ferrell, 2009), social understanding, rather than egocentrism, provides a more parsimonious explanation of children’s intergroup bias.  相似文献   
137.
A total of 247 American children between 6 and 10 years of age (126 girls and 121 boys) completed Implicit Association Tests and explicit self-report measures assessing the association of (a) me with male (gender identity), (b) male with math (math-gender stereotype), and (c) me with math (math self-concept). Two findings emerged. First, as early as second grade, the children demonstrated the American cultural stereotype that math is for boys on both implicit and explicit measures. Second, elementary school boys identified with math more strongly than did girls on both implicit and self-report measures. The findings suggest that the math-gender stereotype is acquired early and influences emerging math self-concepts prior to ages at which there are actual differences in math achievement.  相似文献   
138.
Male and female infants from rural Ethiopia were tested to investigate relations among hemoglobin (Hb), anthropometry, and attention. A longitudinal design was used to examine differences in attention performance from 6 (M = 24.9 weeks, n = 89) to 9 months of age (M = 40.6 weeks, n = 85), differences hypothesized to be related to changes in iron status and growth delays. Stunting (length‐for‐age z scores < ?2.0) and attention performance, t(30) = ?2.42, p = .022, worsened over time. Growth and Hb predicted attention at 9 months, R2 = .15, p < .05, but not at 6. The study contributes to the knowledge base concerning the relations among Hb, early growth, and attention.  相似文献   
139.
The purpose of this study was to explore the utility of a dynamic assessment (DA) of algebraic learning in predicting third graders' development of mathematics word-problem difficulty. In the fall, 122 third-grade students were assessed on a test of math word-problem skill and DA of algebraic learning. In the spring, they were assessed on word-problem performance. Logistic regression was conducted to contrast two models. One relied exclusively on the fall test of math word-problem skill to predict word-problem difficulty on the spring outcome (less than the 25th percentile). The second model relied on a combination of the fall test of math word-problem skill and the fall DA to predict the same outcome. Holding sensitivity at 87.5%, the universal screener alone resulted in a high proportion of false positives, which was practically reduced when DA was included in the prediction model. Findings are discussed in terms of a two-stage process for screening students within a responsiveness-to-intervention prevention model.  相似文献   
140.
Children with learning problems require early intervention. If it is evidence based and implemented with integrity and intensity, it will accelerate the academic progress of many students. This is the hope and expectation of the many supporters of responsiveness-to-intervention (RTI). A minority of children, however, will not respond sufficiently to such intervention because of learning disorders like specific learning disabilities (SLD). Some RTI models do not include research-backed methods to identify these children, nor do RTI practitioners often produce the data necessary to develop individualized instruction for them. The authors suggest practitioners go beyond typical RTI assessment data documenting responsiveness/ unresponsiveness to conduct comprehensive evaluations of these most difficult-to-teach students and to include in their evaluations carefully chosen cognitive measures. This special issue presents the work of teams of researchers, which suggests that cognitive and neuropsychological assessments can provide information to further understand SLD, which in turn can guide development of promising interventions.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号