首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1519篇
  免费   12篇
  国内免费   20篇
教育   813篇
科学研究   224篇
各国文化   4篇
体育   339篇
综合类   20篇
文化理论   2篇
信息传播   149篇
  2024年   8篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   14篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   5篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   3篇
  2017年   3篇
  2016年   6篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   33篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   127篇
  2011年   140篇
  2010年   62篇
  2009年   39篇
  2008年   86篇
  2007年   117篇
  2006年   103篇
  2005年   105篇
  2004年   75篇
  2003年   77篇
  2002年   56篇
  2001年   44篇
  2000年   41篇
  1999年   28篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   23篇
  1995年   14篇
  1994年   18篇
  1993年   19篇
  1992年   21篇
  1991年   23篇
  1990年   12篇
  1989年   11篇
  1988年   16篇
  1987年   14篇
  1986年   19篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   17篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   4篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   3篇
  1958年   3篇
  1948年   7篇
排序方式: 共有1551条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
111.
航空和社会     
飞机是否飞得太高了?这个不必回答、只是为了加强印象的反问句表明这样一种关切:今天飞机的航线并不象从前的商业和旅行的非常大的交通干线——沙漠和海洋——一样,也许不会对航线通过的国家的民族和文化产生相对来说较深远的影响。诚然,飞机用于空中运输正在不断地使地球变小,而飞机用于空中作业,创造了许多人道主义的或科学上的奇迹。“但是”,作者问道,“飞机是否深入到了人的内心世界?是否改变了人的心理活动或丰富了人的文化生活?”  相似文献   
112.
Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show differences in face processing abilities from early in development. To examine whether these differences reflect an atypical versus delayed developmental trajectory, neural responses to familiar and unfamiliar faces in twenty-four 18- to 47-month-old children with ASD were compared with responses of thirty-two 12- to 30-month-old typically developing children. Results of 2 experiments revealed that neural responses to faces in children with ASD resembled those observed in younger typically developing children, suggesting delayed development. Electrophysiological responses to faces were also related to parent report of adaptive social behaviors for both children with ASD and typical development. Slower development of the face processing system in ASD may be related to reduced self-directed "expected" experience with faces in early development.  相似文献   
113.
Male and female infants from rural Ethiopia were tested to investigate relations among hemoglobin (Hb), anthropometry, and attention. A longitudinal design was used to examine differences in attention performance from 6 (M = 24.9 weeks, n = 89) to 9 months of age (M = 40.6 weeks, n = 85), differences hypothesized to be related to changes in iron status and growth delays. Stunting (length‐for‐age z scores < ?2.0) and attention performance, t(30) = ?2.42, p = .022, worsened over time. Growth and Hb predicted attention at 9 months, R2 = .15, p < .05, but not at 6. The study contributes to the knowledge base concerning the relations among Hb, early growth, and attention.  相似文献   
114.
The authors used a large sample of children (N ≈ 7,400) participating in the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study--Kindergarten Cohort (ECLS-K) to estimate kindergarten children's academic achievement growth trajectories in reading and mathematics. The authors were particularly interested in whether the growth trajectories of children with learning disabilities (LD) or speech language impairments (SLI)--as well as those of other groups of children--were consistent with a cumulative or compensatory developmental cycle. Both LD and SLI children displayed significantly lower levels of kindergarten reading achievement than nondisabled children. However, and over the subsequent 5 years of elementary school, only children with SLI lagged increasingly behind nondisabled peers in their reading skills growth. The authors observed a different pattern for mathematics achievement. Children with LD, but not SLI, lagged increasingly behind nondisabled children in their mathematics skills growth. The authors also observed some consistency in "poor-get-poorer" effects across reading and mathematic achievement for additional population subgroups. Those kindergarten children who were from lower socioeconomic status families, who were African American, and who more frequently displayed learning-related behavior problems initially had lower levels of reading and mathematics achievement and also lagged increasingly behind in their acquisition of these skills over time. Some groups of children, including those with SLI, experience a cumulative rather than a compensatory cycle of achievement growth.  相似文献   
115.

Objective

We sought to determine the incidence, clinical features, and demographic profile of head injury secondary to suspected child maltreatment (abuse or neglect) in Canada to help inform the development and evaluation of prevention programs for abusive head injuries.

Methods

From March 1, 2005 to February 28, 2008, an average of 2,545 paediatricians and paediatric subspecialists were surveyed monthly through the established network of the Canadian Paediatric Surveillance Program. We calculated incidence rates using the number of confirmed cases over the product of the duration of the study (3 years) and population estimates by age group.

Results

There were 220 confirmed cases of head injury from suspected child maltreatment. The annual incidence rate was 14.1 per 100,000 for children less than 1 year of age and 1.4 per 100,000 for those less than 15 years. Seventy three percent (141) of cases involved infants less than 12 months of age and 52% (100) of cases involved infants less than 6 months of age. Seventy-five percent (165) of cases presented to the emergency room. With regard to outcome, 12% (27) of cases resulted in death and 45% (75) of survivors had neurological sequelae at discharge. Thirty percent (67) of all cases, as well as 30% (8) of deaths were previously known to child welfare authorities.

Conclusion

This study provides an estimate of the rate of head injury secondary to suspected child maltreatment in Canada. The young age and poor medical outcomes of those involved highlights the need for prevention efforts that are implemented early in life. Given that a significant percentage of injured infants and children were already known to child welfare authorities, the study also highlights the need to establish and evaluate additional preventive efforts for parents and caregivers already in the child welfare system.  相似文献   
116.

Objectives

During intrafamilial conflicts children are often innocent bystanders, caught in the crossfire. In such situations, they are at increased risk to become directly involved in abusive verbal behavior of the perpetrator, and exposed to being shouted or yelled at, threatened, rejected and even physically abused. The present study has two main objectives: (1) ascertain a national base rate of intrafamilial conflicts and physical violence at home among Icelandic adolescents; and (2) to investigate the association of witnessing and/or having been a part of intrafamilial conflict or physical violence at home with variables that relate to mental health and well-being.

Methods

The participants were 3,515 students, 14- and 15-year-old, in the national compulsory school system in Iceland. As a part of the 2003 ESPAD survey, each pupil was asked about experiences of severe verbal arguments and physical violence at home as well as their background, behaviors, and mental health assessed with the use of tested measurement scales such as the Symptom Distress Checklist 90 (SCL-90) and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale.

Results

About 22% of the participants stated that they had witnessed a severe verbal argument between parents and 34% stated that they had been involved in a severe verbal argument with parents. This rate was slightly higher for girls compared to boys. All together 7% of adolescents had witnessed physical violence at home where an adult was involved and 6% of the participants stated that they had experiences of being involved in physical violence at home where an adult was involved. Witnessing or being involved in severe verbal arguments at home and/or witnessing or being involved in physical violence with an adult was significantly associated with greater levels of depression, anger, and anxiety, and negatively related with self-esteem (p < 0.01).

Conclusions

Many adolescents in Iceland witness severe parental verbal arguments or physical violence between adults in their homes and some are directly involved in such acts. It affects their long-term emotional and behavioral development and well-being.

Practice implications

Preventive measures have to be implemented at an early age and should include, but not be limited to, information on disciplining and upbringing of children and the negative impact of intrafamilial conflicts on the long-term health of their children. Due attention should be given to the health and well-being of children where such violence is known to occur.  相似文献   
117.
从高职院校图书馆组织网络文献现实状况入手,指出对于网络全文数据库高职图书馆缺乏网络文献组织技术手段,对于组织开放获取资源没有积极性的问题.分析高职图书馆开展网络文献组织的好处有以下几个方面,跨库统一检索平台可以有效将图书馆现有文献组织成一个整体,开放获取文献可以缓解图书馆经费压力,组织网络文献可以缓解读者信息过载.并指出高职院校图书馆网络文献组织工作切入点有图书馆现有网络资源组织统一检索平,组织开放获取资源向读者推广使用,接受虚拟馆藏文献概念,积极参与网络文献资源建设.  相似文献   
118.
杜亮  刘涛  赵飞  华岩 《图书馆学刊》2011,(10):46-48
"]十二五"时期,数字资源建设与共享成为图书馆发展的核心与关键。从数字资源建设的宏观布局入手,提出建立"三级互补"的保障体制。数字资源建设的核心是数据库建设,要加强数据库建设的统筹规划、标准化和规范化,加大投资力度,推动数据库建设的多样化发展;另外,通过建立健全数字资源共享管理体系,加强数字资源共享网络的软、硬件建设,最终实现垂直型、水平型和网络型3种共享模式。  相似文献   
119.
Using the National Science Foundation's 2006 Science Indicators Survey, this study explores three distinct explanations of public attitudes. First, the knowledge-attitudes model refers to a well tested relationship between public knowledge of science and more favorable attitudes toward science. Second, the alienation model hypothesizes that public disassociation with science is a symptom of a general disenchantment with late modernity, mainly, the limitations associated with codified expertise, rational bureaucracy, and institutional authority. A third approach emphasizes the cultural meaning of science: how various public beliefs about "what science is" relate to acceptance or reservations about science. The Science Indicators Survey shows that US adults view science (what it is or should be) in three distinct ways: (1) in terms of having a systematic method, (2) in terms of social location (i.e., takes place in a university or a laboratory), and (3) in terms of knowledge that should accord with commonsense and tradition. The findings in this study indicate that the knowledge-attitudes, alienation, and cultural meanings models are all valuable for understanding the cultural authority of science. However, the strength of these explanations depends on the type of attitude analyzed.  相似文献   
120.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号