首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2924篇
  免费   21篇
  国内免费   23篇
教育   1602篇
科学研究   445篇
各国文化   3篇
体育   592篇
综合类   51篇
文化理论   13篇
信息传播   262篇
  2024年   8篇
  2023年   12篇
  2022年   28篇
  2021年   10篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   18篇
  2015年   22篇
  2014年   40篇
  2013年   45篇
  2012年   206篇
  2011年   298篇
  2010年   117篇
  2009年   40篇
  2008年   203篇
  2007年   196篇
  2006年   222篇
  2005年   166篇
  2004年   149篇
  2003年   141篇
  2002年   127篇
  2001年   88篇
  2000年   89篇
  1999年   84篇
  1998年   16篇
  1997年   30篇
  1996年   23篇
  1995年   27篇
  1994年   35篇
  1993年   37篇
  1992年   31篇
  1991年   30篇
  1990年   27篇
  1989年   42篇
  1988年   16篇
  1987年   28篇
  1986年   34篇
  1985年   32篇
  1984年   19篇
  1983年   21篇
  1982年   26篇
  1981年   16篇
  1959年   11篇
  1958年   9篇
  1957年   9篇
  1956年   11篇
  1955年   9篇
  1954年   10篇
排序方式: 共有2968条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
启发式教学是我们常见的教学方法,为了适应21世纪社会主义现代化建设需要,对材料化学本科教育重新进行了定位,从教学中激发学生的学习兴趣。本文论述了在材料化学专业课中开展启发式教学的必要性和现实意义,并探讨了运用启发式教学激发学生学习动机,培养科学思维方法,从而推动材料化学本科教育的发展。  相似文献   
22.
凡实验室涉及到生物学和化学的水溶液体系时,pH和离子强度(I)是两个极为重要的可变量。由于酸—碱混合物的存在,这两个参量往往相互依赖。控制pH值的通常方法是使用缓冲剂。它们是由那些对于溶液离子行为影响不大、且不活泼的稳定电解质来配制的。本文提出的方法适用于计算某种酸及其盐混合物的pH值和离子强度;或与此相反,用于计算配制预知pH值和离子强度(I)所需酸及其盐的相当量。这种计算方法很实用,具有启发性和直观教学的价值,在许多普通的可编程序计算器上容易完成。  相似文献   
23.
We first report discovery of the spinel-garnet-orthopyroxene granulite with pure CO2 fluid inclusions from the Fuyun region of the late Paleozoic Altay orogenic belt in Central Asia, NW China. The rock is characterized by an assemblage of garnet, orthopyroxene, spinel, cordierite, biotite, plagioclase and quartz. Symplectites of orthopyroxene and spinel, and orthopyroxene and cordierite indicate decompression under UHT conditions. Mineral chemistry shows that the or-thopyroxenes have high XMg and A12O3 contents (up to 9.23 wt%). Biotites are enriched in TiO2 and XMg and are stable under granulite facies conditions. The garnet and quartz from the rock carry monophase fluid inclusions which show peak melting temperatures of around -56.7℃, indicating a pure CO2 species being presented during the ultrahigh-T metamorphism in the Altay orogenic belt. The inclusions homogenize into a liquid phase at temperatures around 15.3-23.8℃ translating into CO2 densities of the order of 0.86-0.88 g/cm3. Based on prelim  相似文献   
24.
高校辅导员是高等学校大学生日常性思想政治教育的骨干,对于其非物化工作成效的考核是辅导员人力资源开发的重点和难点。从胜任力素质理论出发,对辅导员的素质特征、工作行为、工作成效进行科学的考评认定。从辅导员胜任力素质分析入手,探讨辅导员胜任力素质视角下辅导员绩效考核、管理的理论与方法,建构辅导员胜任力模型的绩效考核体系,解决理论与现实的双重困惑。  相似文献   
25.
小女孩辛迪和她的爸爸妈妈生活在一起。她的家庭并不富裕,但一家人非常和睦,生活也很幸福。然而生活中好像还存在着什么缺憾,辛迪没意识到。辛迪9岁那年,第一次在外面过夜,那是在她的好朋友黛比家。睡觉前黛比的妈妈进来,分别轻轻地吻了吻她们的额头,并向她们道了“晚安”。“我爱你,黛比。”当黛比的妈妈吻过黛比之后,轻声地说道。“我也爱您,妈妈。”黛比喃喃地应道。面对此情此景,辛迪不禁大吃一惊,心中久久不能平静。从她记事以来,从来就没有人吻过她,还向她道晚安、说爱她之类的话。整整一夜,她的眼前始终闪现着黛比的妈妈轻吻黛比时的…  相似文献   
26.
The authors examined second grade reading accuracy and fluency and their associations via letter knowledge to phonological and language predictors assessed at 3.5, 4.5, and 5.5 years in children in the Jyv?skyl? Longitudinal Study of Dyslexia. Structural equation modeling showed that a developmentally highly stable factor (early phonological and language processing [EPLP]) behind key dyslexia predictors (i.e., phonological awareness, short-term memory, rapid naming, vocabulary, and pseudoword repetition) could already be identified at 3.5 years. EPLP was significantly associated with reading and spelling accuracy and by age with letter knowledge. However, EPLP had only a minor link with reading fluency, which was additionally explained by early letter knowledge. The results show that reading accuracy is well predicted by early phonological and language skills. Variation in fluent reading skills is not well explained by early skills, suggesting factors other than phonological core skills. Future research is suggested to explore the factors behind the development of fast and accurate decoding skills.  相似文献   
27.
Early developmental psychologists viewed iconic representation as cognitively less complex than other forms of symbolic thought. It is therefore surprising that iconic signs are not acquired more easily than arbitrary signs by young language learners. One explanation is that children younger than 3 years have difficulty interpreting iconicity. The current study assessed hearing children's ability to interpret the meaning of iconic signs. Sixty-six 2.5- to 5-year-olds who had no previous exposure to signs were required to match iconic signs to pictures of referents. Whereas few of the 2.5-year-olds recognized the meaning of the iconic signs consistently, more than half of the 3.0-year-olds and most of 3.5-year-olds performed above chance. Thus, the ability to recognize the meaning of iconic signs gradually develops during the preschool years. Implications of these findings for sign language development, receptive signed vocabulary tests, and the development of the ability to interpret iconic symbols are discussed.  相似文献   
28.
OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this study is to examine processing of facial emotions in a sample of maltreated children showing high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Maltreatment during childhood has been associated independently with both atypical processing of emotion and the development of PTSD. However, research has provided little evidence indicating how high rates of PTSD might relate to maltreated children's processing of emotions. METHOD: Participants' reaction time and labeling of emotions were measured using a morphed facial emotion identification task. Participants included a diverse sample of maltreated children with and without PTSD and controls ranging in age from 8 to 15 years. Maltreated children had been removed from their homes and placed in state custody following experiences of maltreatment. Diagnoses of PTSD and other disorders were determined through combination of parent, child, and teacher reports. RESULTS: Maltreated children displayed faster reaction times than controls when labeling emotional facial expressions, and this result was most pronounced for fearful faces. Relative to children who were not maltreated, maltreated children both with and without PTSD showed enhanced response times when identifying fearful faces. There was no group difference in labeling of emotions when identifying different facial emotions. CONCLUSIONS: Maltreated children show heightened ability to identify fearful faces, evidenced by faster reaction times relative to controls. This association between maltreatment and atypical processing of emotion is independent of PTSD diagnosis.  相似文献   
29.
"孩子,快12点了,睡觉吧!" 看到上初三的女儿做完作业,又疲惫地拿起语文课本在默读,我心疼地说. "妈妈,过来,提问!" "提问什么?"她今天学的是古文《陈涉世家》,书上圈圈点点,我一脸迷茫. "注释." 我仔细一看,文下的主要注释都打上小钩钩. "老师说了,这些词明天上课测试,一个字都不能错."  相似文献   
30.
INTRODUCTION Because of its low molecular weight and ability to fluoresce independently (George, 1997), the new molecular tag, green fluorescent protein (GFP), has become more and more popular after Prasher et al.(1992) cloned its cDNA in 1992. There are many reports describing the co-expression of GFP and a specific antibody or cytokine gene, with the fusion protein expressing the fluorescent activity and bio-logical activity of the complement protein (Haraguchi et al., 1999; Mclean…  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号