首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3992篇
  免费   49篇
  国内免费   4篇
教育   2918篇
科学研究   271篇
各国文化   70篇
体育   361篇
综合类   1篇
文化理论   37篇
信息传播   387篇
  2023年   14篇
  2022年   21篇
  2021年   42篇
  2020年   69篇
  2019年   115篇
  2018年   138篇
  2017年   154篇
  2016年   134篇
  2015年   92篇
  2014年   127篇
  2013年   914篇
  2012年   117篇
  2011年   101篇
  2010年   86篇
  2009年   102篇
  2008年   122篇
  2007年   111篇
  2006年   103篇
  2005年   98篇
  2004年   90篇
  2003年   75篇
  2002年   78篇
  2001年   52篇
  2000年   62篇
  1999年   62篇
  1998年   45篇
  1997年   44篇
  1996年   62篇
  1995年   47篇
  1994年   53篇
  1993年   37篇
  1992年   50篇
  1991年   36篇
  1990年   45篇
  1989年   36篇
  1988年   29篇
  1987年   24篇
  1986年   27篇
  1985年   25篇
  1984年   21篇
  1983年   15篇
  1982年   25篇
  1981年   29篇
  1980年   27篇
  1979年   18篇
  1978年   21篇
  1976年   19篇
  1975年   14篇
  1974年   20篇
  1973年   15篇
排序方式: 共有4045条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
This study investigated interactions between the isolated–interactive elements effect and levels of learner expertise with first year undergraduate university accounting students. The isolated–interactive elements effect occurs when learning is facilitated by initially presenting elements of information sequentially in an isolated form rather than in a fully interactive form. The expertise reversal effect occurs when the relevant advantage of one instructional technique over another reverses depending on the learner’s level of expertise. The results provided support for the predicted interaction with lower expertise students benefitting from the isolated elements instructional method, while students with more expertise learned more from the interacting elements format.  相似文献   
992.
This study reports findings on the relative effects from a yearlong secondary intervention contrasting large-group, small-group, and school-provided interventions emphasizing word study, vocabulary development, fluency, and comprehension with seventh- and eighth-graders with reading difficulties. Findings indicate that few statistically significant results or clinically significant gains were associated with group size or intervention. Findings also indicate that a significant acceleration of reading outcomes for seventh- and eighth-graders from high-poverty schools is unlikely to result from a 50 min daily class. Instead, the findings indicate, achieving this outcome will require more comprehensive models including more extensive intervention (e.g., more time, even smaller groups), interventions that are longer in duration (multiple years), and interventions that vary in emphasis based on specific students’ needs (e.g., increased focus on comprehension or word study).  相似文献   
993.
994.
995.
996.
For a decade or so there has been a new ‘hype’ in educational research: it is called educational neuroscience or even neuroeducation (and neuroethics)—there are numerous publications, special journals, and an abundance of research projects together with the advertisement of many positions at renowned research centres worldwide. After a brief introduction of what is going on in the ‘emerging sub‐discipline’, a number of characterisations are offered of what is envisaged by authors working in this field. In the discussion that follows various problems are listed: the assumption that ‘visual proof’ of brain activity is supposedly given; the correlational nature of this kind of research; the nature of the concepts that are used; the lack of addressing and possibly influencing the neurological mechanism; and finally the need for other insights in educational contexts. Following Bakhurst and others, a number of crucially relevant philosophical issues are highlighted. It is argued that though there are cases where neuroscience insights may be helpful, these are scarce. In general, it is concluded, not a lot may be expected from this discipline for education and educational research. A reminder is offered that the promise of neurophilia may be just another neuromyth, which needs to be addressed by philosophy and education.  相似文献   
997.
Research in Science Education - In science education, there is a now established focus on fostering students’ meaning making through/as multimodal representations as part of their induction...  相似文献   
998.
Higher Education - A worldwide trend towards high levels of participation in higher education, paired with concerns about the post-university destinations of an increasing pool of graduates, have...  相似文献   
999.
Cold comforts     
Warde  Paul 《Metascience》2020,29(2):309-311
  相似文献   
1000.
Educational assessment involves eliciting, transmitting, and receiving information concerning the level of proficiency of a learner in a specified domain. With that in mind, it is perhaps surprising that the literature seems to make very little use of the signal processing metaphor. The present article begins by making a general case for greater use of this metaphor, as a simple and intuitive thinking tool for helping to explain how educational assessment works. The main body of the article extends this argument by demonstrating the utility of the metaphor in helping to explain how educational assessment can go wrong. During the 1980s and 1990s, Samuel Messick extensively discussed two major ways in which educational assessment can go wrong via construct-irrelevant variance and construct underrepresentation, respectively. Despite their importance, these concepts have had only a limited impact on the literature. Part of the reason for this may be a lack of clarity and comprehensiveness in their articulation. The present article aims to articulate these concepts more clearly and comprehensively, within a framework provided by the signal processing metaphor, reconfiguring them as signal contamination and signal deficiency, respectively.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号