首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4079篇
  免费   78篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   2968篇
科学研究   310篇
各国文化   34篇
体育   283篇
综合类   9篇
文化理论   24篇
信息传播   530篇
  2022年   35篇
  2021年   44篇
  2020年   64篇
  2019年   110篇
  2018年   140篇
  2017年   170篇
  2016年   158篇
  2015年   100篇
  2014年   136篇
  2013年   798篇
  2012年   115篇
  2011年   136篇
  2010年   117篇
  2009年   103篇
  2008年   119篇
  2007年   107篇
  2006年   83篇
  2005年   77篇
  2004年   86篇
  2003年   61篇
  2002年   58篇
  2001年   42篇
  2000年   65篇
  1999年   64篇
  1998年   45篇
  1997年   40篇
  1996年   41篇
  1995年   41篇
  1994年   30篇
  1993年   45篇
  1992年   42篇
  1991年   47篇
  1990年   66篇
  1989年   54篇
  1988年   48篇
  1987年   29篇
  1986年   41篇
  1985年   44篇
  1984年   39篇
  1983年   31篇
  1982年   32篇
  1981年   24篇
  1980年   23篇
  1979年   36篇
  1978年   32篇
  1977年   25篇
  1976年   21篇
  1974年   22篇
  1971年   18篇
  1968年   22篇
排序方式: 共有4158条查询结果,搜索用时 14 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
64.
To adequately monitor progress in writing, a test must show stability and demonstrate a performance profile over time that parallels those of accepted criterion measures. This study investigated the technical adequacy of seven objective indexes of writing quality in monitoring the progress of 36 middle school (Grades 6-8) students with mild disabilities over a 6-month period. The stability of each index was assessed. Holistic ratings of the same writing samples and the Test of Written Language served as validation criteria. Three indexes were moderately correlated with holistic ratings, but were not sufficiently stable over time. Direct, objective writing assessment must be used with caution for progress monitoring, given our present lack of precision in measuring the complex task of writing.  相似文献   
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
Extracting policy-relevant information from large national surveys of educational achievement is ordinarily a nontrivial task. It is made more treacherous when the data are expressed on scales that are not uniquely determined. The paper begins with a critical analysis of a recent attempt to interpret the findings on reading achievement obtained by the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). It then describes a new approach to the quantification and interpretation of change and demonstrates its appropriateness for repeated cross-sectional designs such as NAEP. Limitations imposed by the survey design and the nature of the measurements are highlighted  相似文献   
70.
Rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) were given either a 100% or a 50% reinforcement schedule in classical conditioning. Two groups received an aversive US (shock) and two groups received an appetitive US (water to the oral cavity). With parameter estimates for the two-phase model serving as the dependent variables, it was possible to define more precisely the effect of US omission with the intermittent reinforcement schedule. For the aversive preparation, the major effect of intermittent reinforcement was to increase the duration of Phase 1, a phase during which response likelihood remains constant at its initial value. Only a small proportion of subjects reflected the effect of US omission during Phase 2, the “learning” phase, this being a low operator limit following trials on which neither the CR nor the US occurred. The major effect of US omission for the appetitive preparation was in Phase 2, primarily a result of a minority of subjects having a low operator limit following trials on which neither the CR nor the US occurred. Many subjects required separate operators for trials on which the CR did or did not occur. The results were interpreted to pose difficulties for strength theories of conditioning, and the limitation implied for successful application of the Rescorla-Wagner theory are discussed. The implications of the data for response-contingent interpretations and for individual differences are also discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号