首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2994篇
  免费   56篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   2152篇
科学研究   181篇
各国文化   30篇
体育   417篇
文化理论   77篇
信息传播   194篇
  2023年   27篇
  2022年   46篇
  2021年   71篇
  2020年   124篇
  2019年   223篇
  2018年   262篇
  2017年   245篇
  2016年   203篇
  2015年   137篇
  2014年   117篇
  2013年   674篇
  2012年   108篇
  2011年   84篇
  2010年   66篇
  2009年   46篇
  2008年   52篇
  2007年   50篇
  2006年   52篇
  2005年   36篇
  2004年   24篇
  2003年   34篇
  2002年   18篇
  2001年   25篇
  2000年   19篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   15篇
  1997年   13篇
  1996年   18篇
  1995年   20篇
  1994年   14篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   14篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   17篇
  1989年   9篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   9篇
  1986年   11篇
  1985年   8篇
  1984年   5篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   10篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   7篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   5篇
  1870年   4篇
排序方式: 共有3051条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
101.
102.
103.
104.
105.
106.
The aims of this study were twofold: (1) to determine whether open feedback (i.e. the ability to hear or see the scores of colleague judges after each performance) would lead judges in gymnastics to conform with their colleagues, and (2) to identify the underlying process on which this conformity is based. Twenty-three certified Flemish judges in women's gymnastics were randomly divided into panels of a maximum of five judges. These panels had to rate the same 30 videotaped individual vaults: 15 in phase 1 and 15 in phase 2. Two independent variables were orthogonally manipulated: feedback (or no feedback) during phase 1 and feedback (or no feedback) during phase 2. The results of phase 1 revealed that the variation between the judges' scores was less within panels that had received feedback than within panels that had not received feedback. We therefore conclude that the availability of feedback elicits conformity among gymnastic judges. The results of phase 2 indicated that this conformity continued even when feedback was no longer provided, suggesting that the observed conformity was based on informational influencing (i.e. because of uncertainty about the correct responses) and not on normative influencing (i.e. out of fear of standing out in the group).  相似文献   
107.
The aims of this study were to investigate the energy build-up and dissipation mechanisms associated with using an arm swing in submaximal and maximal vertical jumping and to establish the energy benefit of this arm swing. Twenty adult males were asked to perform a series of submaximal and maximal vertical jumps while using an arm swing. Force, motion and electromyographic data were recorded during each performance and used to compute a range of kinematic and kinetic variables, including ankle, knee, hip, shoulder and elbow joint powers and work done. It was found that the energy benefit of using an arm swing appears to be closely related to the maximum kinetic energy of the arms during their downswing, and increases as jump height increases. As jump height increases, energy in the arms is built up by a greater range of motion at the shoulder and greater effort of the shoulder and elbow muscles but, as jump height approaches maximum, these sources are supplemented by energy supplied by the trunk due to its earlier extension in the movement. The kinetic energy developed by the arms is used to increase their potential energy at take-off but also to store and return energy from the lower limbs and to "pull" on the rest of the body. These latter two mechanisms become more important as jump height increases with the pull being the more important of the two. We conclude that an arm swing contributes to jump performance in submaximal as well as maximal jumping but the energy generation and dissipation sources change as performance approaches maximum.  相似文献   
108.
109.
110.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号