首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   405篇
  免费   9篇
教育   262篇
科学研究   40篇
各国文化   7篇
体育   35篇
文化理论   6篇
信息传播   64篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   10篇
  2020年   26篇
  2019年   27篇
  2018年   39篇
  2017年   30篇
  2016年   32篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   89篇
  2012年   16篇
  2011年   12篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   8篇
  2008年   10篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   4篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   8篇
  2002年   7篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   3篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   1篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   2篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
  1918年   5篇
  1833年   2篇
  1830年   1篇
排序方式: 共有414条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
411.
数字图像分析方法在螺纹检测中的应用   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
This paper describes the use of computer-aided measurement for external metric screw threads.Thread parameters,including thread pitch,thread angle,pitch giameter and major diameter,were measured with CCD cameras and image analysis software.New technologies such as digital image processing were used to increase the efficiency of measurements.In this study,by reconstructing the toolmaker‘s microscope,the computer-aided semi-automated measuring system was developed,which could evaluate the accuracy of screw thread fpofile.It is concluded that the measuement accuracy is comparable to that of traditional toolmaker‘s microscope method.  相似文献   
412.
The idea that characteristics acquired by an organism during its lifetime can be inherited by offspring and result in evolution is a substantial impediment to student understanding of evolution. In the current study, we performed a preliminary examination of how acquiring physical changes in a question prompt may differentially cue intuitive and scientific justifications of inheritance and evolution and how this varies based on how student learned the concept. Middle school students in a suburban northeastern district (N = 314) either learned about evolutionary change with a category construction task (with different levels of feedback support) or completed a worksheet. Three days later students responded to two free response scenarios (one where a physical change is acquired). Responses were coded based on student justifications for either science accuracy or intuitive nature. Specific reasons were coded by justification type with high inter-rater agreement (k > 0.93). Results showed that students were more likely to apply intuitive reasoning when a physical change was acquired (50%) than if the change was behavioral in nature (16%). Additionally, students who completed the category construction task provided significantly more scientifically accurate justifications about inheritance (M = 1.12) than control students (M = 0.47), and significantly less intuitive justifications (M = 0.67) than control (M = 1.13). Finally, category construction produced the most scientific reasoning when feedback was provided. Taken together, these results suggest that intuitive reasoning is differentially applied based on physical organismal changes, intuitive reasoning is less frequent when learning via category construction, and the category construction task is more effective for this population with the inclusion of feedback.  相似文献   
413.
During feature-positive operant discriminations, a conditional cue, X, signals whether responses made during a second stimulus, A, are reinforced. Few studies have examined how landmarks, which can be trained to control the spatial distribution of responses during search tasks, might operate under conditional control. We trained college students to search for a target hidden on a computer monitor. Participants learned that responses to a hidden target location signaled by a landmark (e.g., A) would be reinforced only if the landmark was preceded by a colored background display (e.g., X). In Experiment 1, participants received feature-positive training (+←YB/ XA→+/A?/B?) with the hidden target to the right of A and to left of B. Responding during nonreinforced transfer test trials (XB?/YA?) indicated conditional control by the colored background, and spatial accuracy indicated a greater weighting of spatial information provided by the landmark than by the conditional cue. In Experiments 2a and 2b, the location of the target relative to landmark A was conditional on the colored background (+←YA/ XA→+/ ZB→+/ +←C /A?/B?). At test, conditional control and a greater weighting for the landmark’s spatial information were again found, but we also report evidence for spatial interference by the conditional stimulus. Overall, we found that hierarchical accounts best explain the observed differences in response magnitude, whereas spatial accuracy was best explained via spatial learning models that emphasize the reliability, stability, and proximity of landmarks to a target.  相似文献   
414.
Abstract Interactive museum exhibits are ubiquitous in science centers, and are becoming increasingly popular in art, history and cultural museums. At an interactive exhibit, visitors can act on the exhibit and the exhibit reacts. While there is much theoretical and empirical support for the idea that interactive features promote science learning, we believe that serious design problems can arise if an uncritical “more is better” approach is taken to interactivity. This article describes five common pitfalls of designing exhibits with high levels of interactivity or multiple interactive features: 1) multiple options with equal salience, 2) features allowing multiple users to interfere with one another, 3) options that encourage users to disrupt the phenomenon being displayed, 4) features that make the critical phenomenon difficult to find, and 5) secondary features that obscure the primary feature. Examples of each of the five problems are presented, and possible design solutions are offered.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号