首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   22678篇
  免费   324篇
  国内免费   27篇
教育   15641篇
科学研究   2536篇
各国文化   454篇
体育   1870篇
综合类   11篇
文化理论   164篇
信息传播   2353篇
  2021年   238篇
  2020年   352篇
  2019年   490篇
  2018年   628篇
  2017年   632篇
  2016年   595篇
  2015年   406篇
  2014年   509篇
  2013年   3997篇
  2012年   508篇
  2011年   530篇
  2010年   453篇
  2009年   406篇
  2008年   500篇
  2007年   447篇
  2006年   411篇
  2005年   380篇
  2004年   415篇
  2003年   332篇
  2002年   294篇
  2001年   578篇
  2000年   433篇
  1999年   387篇
  1998年   202篇
  1997年   234篇
  1996年   209篇
  1995年   191篇
  1994年   208篇
  1993年   193篇
  1992年   310篇
  1991年   324篇
  1990年   321篇
  1989年   340篇
  1988年   276篇
  1987年   265篇
  1986年   265篇
  1985年   347篇
  1984年   278篇
  1983年   253篇
  1982年   225篇
  1981年   196篇
  1980年   200篇
  1979年   289篇
  1978年   206篇
  1977年   194篇
  1976年   166篇
  1975年   182篇
  1974年   177篇
  1973年   181篇
  1971年   164篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
961.
962.
963.
964.
965.
This study examined data from over 300 U.S. schools on their efforts to develop high quality programs of school, family, and community partnerships. Analyses show that elementary schools, schools with greater support from parents, teachers, and the community; and schools that evaluated progress reported higher quality partnership programs over time. Higher quality programs were associated with wider implementation of parent-child interactive homework, higher levels of parent volunteering, and more parents on school decision-making committees. Results identify factors that could help schools develop quality partnership programs and suggest that these programs translate into higher levels of family involvement in students’ learning.  相似文献   
966.
967.
968.
Recently, March, Hooper, and Baum (1977) described a sample of 99 nonstudent elders in terms of their demographic characteristics and their attitudes toward lifelong learning. The sample was found to be well‐educated (mean years of education = 13.61, SD = 9.43), active in the community, well‐read, and fairly affluent. The majority demonstrated a quite open attitude toward the appropriateness of formal education in old age, but also felt that learning can be acquired outside the classroom: “living is learning.”

The present study was undertaken to similarly describe a sample of elderly people enrolled in the University of Wisconsin guest student program, and to contrast these respondents with those in the earlier study. The student group was found to be significantly younger than the nonstudent group (mean = 70.78, SD = 7.21 vs. mean = 74.69, SD = 8.45; t = 2.87, p < .001). Although there is no difference in the mean years of education of the two groups, there is significantly greater variability in years of education in the student group (F = 10.98, p < .001), with 50.4% of the nonstudent group versus 21.6% of the student group having 12 years of education or less. Additionally, it was found that only 10 of the 139 student respondents did not have either personal or vicarious personal (through familial significant others) experience with higher education in youth.

The respondents’ stated preference for class format and student group age is also summarized, and some implications for educational gerontology are discussed.  相似文献   
969.
970.
Exploratory activity was examined in 4 young baboons with the aim of investigating the type of spatial coding (purely geometric and/or by taking into account the identity of the object) used for the configuration of objects. Animals were individually tested in an outdoor enclosure for their exploratory reactions (contact time and order of spontaneous visits) to changes brought about to a configuration of different objects. Two kinds of spatial changes were made: a modification (1) of the shape of the configuration (by displacement of one object) and (2) of the spatial arrangement without changing the initial shape (exchanging the location of two objects). In the second experiment, the effect of a spatial modification of the global geometry constituted by four identical objects was investigated. Finally, in the third experiment, a substitution of a familiar object with a novel one was performed without changing the objects’ configuration. The baboons strongly reacted to geometrical modifications of the configuration. In contrast, they were less sensitive to modifications of local features that did not affect the initial spatial configuration. Analyses of spontaneous exploratory activities revealed two types of exploratory strategies (cyclic and back-and-forth). These data are discussed in relation to (1) the distinction between the encoding of geometric versus local spatial features and (2) the spatial function of exploratory activity.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号